U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Kolb H, Fernandez E, Jones B, et al., editors. Webvision: The Organization of the Retina and Visual System [Internet]. Salt Lake City (UT): University of Utah Health Sciences Center; 1995-.

Cover of Webvision

Webvision: The Organization of the Retina and Visual System [Internet].

Show details

Light and Dark Adaptation

and .

Author Information and Affiliations

Created: ; Last Update: July 9, 2007.

Dark Adaptation

The eye operates over a large range of light levels. The sensitivity of our eye can be measured by determining the absolute intensity threshold, that is, the minimum luminance of a test spot required to produce a visual sensation. This can be measured by placing a subject in a dark room and increasing the luminance of the test spot until the subject reports its presence. Consequently, dark adaptation refers to how the eye recovers its sensitivity in the dark after exposure to bright lights. Aubert (1) in 1865 was the first to estimate the threshold stimulus of the eye in the dark by measuring the electrical current required to render the glow on a platinum wire just visible. He found that the sensitivity had increased 35 times after time in the dark, and he also introduced the term "adaptation".

Dark adaptation forms the basis of the Duplicity Theory, which states that above a certain luminance level (about 0.03 cd/m2), the cone mechanism is involved in mediating vision: photopic vision. Below this level, the rod mechanism comes into play, providing scotopic (night) vision. The range where two mechanisms are working together is called the mesopic range, as there is not an abrupt transition between the two mechanisms.

The dark adaptation curve shown in Fig. 1 depicts this duplex nature of our visual system. The first curve reflects the cone mechanism. The sensitivity of the rod pathway improves considerably after 5-10 minutes in the dark and is reflected by the second part of the dark adaptation curve. One way to demonstrate that the rod mechanism takes over at low luminance levels is to observe the color of the stimuli. When the rod mechanism takes over, colored test spots appear colorless, as only the cone pathways encode color. This duplex nature of vision will affect the dark adaptation curve in different ways and is discussed below.

Figure 1. Dark adaptation curve.

Figure 1

Dark adaptation curve. The shaded area represents 80% of the group of subjects. Hecht and Mandelbaum's data are from Pirenne (9).

To produce a dark adaptation curve, subjects gaze at a pre-adapting light for about 5 minutes, and then the absolute threshold is measured over time (Fig. 1). Pre-adaptation is important for normalization and to ensure that a biphasic curve is obtained.

From the above curve, it can be seen that initially there is a rapid decrease in threshold, then it declines slowly. After 5 to 8 minutes, a second mechanism of vision comes into play, where there is another rapid decrease in threshold, then an even slower decline. The curve asymptotes to a minimum (absolute threshold) at about 10−5 cd/m2 after about 40 minutes in the dark.

Factors Affecting Dark Adaptation

There are four factors that affect dark adaptation, which will be discussed below:

1.

intensity and duration of the pre-adapting light

2.

size and position of the retina are used in measuring dark adaptation

3.

wavelength distribution of the light used

4.

rhodopsin regeneration

Intensity and Duration of Pre-adapting Light

Different intensities and duration of the pre-adapting light will affect the dark adaptation curve in a number of areas. With increasing levels of pre-adapting luminances, the cone branch becomes longer while the rod branch becomes more delayed. The absolute threshold also takes longer to reach. At low levels of pre-adapting luminances, rod threshold drops quickly to reach absolute threshold (Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Dark adaptation curves following different levels of pre-adapting luminances.

Figure 2

Dark adaptation curves following different levels of pre-adapting luminances. Hecht, Haig and Chase's data are from Bartlett (10).

The shorter the duration of the pre-adapting light, the more rapid the decrease in dark adaptation (Fig. 3). For extremely short pre-adaptation periods, a single rod curve is obtained. It is only after long pre-adaptation that a biphasic cone and rod branches are obtained.

Figure 3. Dark adaptation curves following different durations of a pre-adapting luminance.

Figure 3

Dark adaptation curves following different durations of a pre-adapting luminance. Wald and Clark's data are from Bartlett (10).

Size and Location of the Retina Used

The retinal location used to register the test spot during dark adaptation will affect the dark adaptation curve because of the distribution of the rod and cones in the retina (Fig. 4).

Figure 4. Graph to show rod and cone densities along the horizontal meridian.

Figure 4

Graph to show rod and cone densities along the horizontal meridian. From Osterberg (11).

When a small test spot is located at the fovea (eccentricity of 0°), only one branch is seen with a higher threshold compared with the rod branch. When the same size test spot is used in the peripheral retina during dark adaptation, the typical break appears in the curve representing the cone branch and the rod branch (Fig. 5).

Figure 5. Dark adaptation measured with a 2° test spot at different angular distances from fixation.

Figure 5

Dark adaptation measured with a 2° test spot at different angular distances from fixation. The subject was pre-adapted for 2 minutes to 300 mL. Hecht, Haig, and Wald's data are from Bartlett (10).

A similar principle applies when different sizes of the test spot is used. When a small test spot is used during dark adaptation, a single branch is found because only cones are present at the fovea. When a larger test spot is used during dark adaptation, a rod-cone break would be present because the test spot stimulates both cones and rods. As the test spot becomes even larger, incorporating more rods, the sensitivity of the eye in the dark is even greater (Fig. 6), reflecting the larger spatial summation characteristics of the rod pathway.

Figure 6. Dark adaptation measured using different size test spots.

Figure 6

Dark adaptation measured using different size test spots. Hecht, Haig, and Wald's data are from Bartlett (10).

Wavelength of the Threshold Light

When stimuli of different wavelengths are used, the dark adaptation curve is affected. In Fig. 7, a rod-cone break is not seen when using light of long wavelengths, such as extreme red. This occurs because of rods and cones having similar sensitivities to light of long wavelengths (Fig. 8). Fig. 8 depicts the photopic and scotopic spectral sensitivity functions to illustrate the point that the rod and cone sensitivity difference is dependent upon test wavelength (although normalization of spatial, temporal, and equivalent adaptation level for the rod and cones is not present in this figure). On the other hand, when light of short wavelength is used, the rod-cone break is most prominent because the rods are much more sensitive than the cones to short wavelengths once the rods have dark adapted.

Figure 7. Dark adaptation curve using different test stimuli of different wavelengths.

Figure 7

Dark adaptation curve using different test stimuli of different wavelengths. Subjects were pre-adapted to 2,000 mL for 5 minutes. A 3° test stimulus was presented 7° on the nasal retina. The colors were: RI (extreme red), 680 nm; RII (red), (more...)

Figure 8. Scotopic (rods) and photopic (cones) spectral sensitivity functions.

Figure 8

Scotopic (rods) and photopic (cones) spectral sensitivity functions. Wald's data from are from Davson (7).

Rhodopsin Regeneration

Dark adaptation also depends upon photopigment bleaching. Retinal (or reflection) densitometry, which is a procedure based on measuring the light reflected from the fundus of the eye, can be used to determine the amount of photopigment bleached. Using retinal densitometry, it was found that the time course for dark adaptation and rhodopsin regeneration was the same. However, this does not fully explain the large increase in sensitivity with time. Bleaching rhodopsin by 1% raises the threshold by 10 (decreases sensitivity by 10). In Fig. 9, it can be seen that bleaching 50% of rhodopsin in rods raises the threshold by 10 log units, whereas bleaching 50% of cone photopigment raises the threshold by about 1.5 log units. Therefore, rod sensitivity is not fully accounted for at the receptor level and may be explained by further retinal processing. The important thing to note is that bleaching of cone photopigment has a smaller effect on cone thresholds.

Figure 9. Log relative threshold as a function of the percentage of photopigment bleached.

Figure 9

Log relative threshold as a function of the percentage of photopigment bleached. From Cornsweet (6).

Light Adaptation

With dark adaptation, we noticed that there is progressive decrease in threshold (increase in sensitivity) with time in the dark. With light adaptation, the eye has to quickly adapt to the background illumination to be able to distinguish objects in this background. Light adaptation can be explored by determining increment thresholds. In an increment threshold experiment, a test stimulus is presented on a background of a certain luminance. The stimulus is increased in luminance until the detection threshold is reached against the background (Fig. 10). Therefore, the independent variable is the luminance of the background, and the dependent variable is the threshold intensity or luminance of the incremental test required for detection. Such an approach is used when visual fields are measured in clinical practice.

Figure 10. Light adaptation using an increment threshold experiment.

Figure 10

Light adaptation using an increment threshold experiment. a, example of the stimulus used. b, luminance profile of the stimulus.

The experimental conditions shown in Fig. 10 can be repeated by changing the background field luminance. Depending upon the choice of test and background wavelength, the test size, and retinal eccentricity, a monophasic or biphasic threshold versus intensity (tvi) curve is obtained. Fig. 11 illustrates such a curve for parafoveal presentation of a yellow test field on a green background. This stimulus choice leads to two branches. A lower branch belongs to the rod system. As the background light level increases, visual function shifts from the rod system to the cone system. A dual-branched curve reflects the duplex nature of vision, similar to the biphasic response in the dark adaptation curve.

Figure 11. Light adaptation curve plotted as increment threshold versus background luminance (or a threshold-versus-intensity: tvi curve).

Figure 11

Light adaptation curve plotted as increment threshold versus background luminance (or a threshold-versus-intensity: tvi curve). The above plot shows increment threshold (Nλ) and background luminance (Mμ). Light of two different wavelengths (more...)

When a single system (e.g., the rod system) is isolated under certain experimental conditions, four sections of the curve are apparent. These experimental conditions involve using a red background to suppress the cone photoreceptors and a green test spot to stimulate the rod photoreceptors (2). The curve in Fig. 12 can also be obtained by performing increment threshold experiments on rod monochromats that lack cone photoreceptors. When the rod system is isolated using the conditions of Aguilar and Stiles (2), four sections are obtained:

Figure 12. Schematic of the increment threshold curve of the rod system.

Figure 12

Schematic of the increment threshold curve of the rod system. Aguilar and Stiles' data are from Davson (7).

1.

dark light

2.

Square Root Law (de Vries-Rose Law)

3.

Weber's Law

4.

saturation

The threshold in the linear portion of the tvi curve is determined by the dark/light level. As background luminance is increased, the curve remains constant (and equal to the absolute threshold). Sensitivity in this section is limited by neural (internal) noise, the so-called "dark light". The background field is relatively low and does not significantly affect threshold. This neural noise is internal to the retina, and examples of these include thermal isomerisations of photopigment, spontaneous opening of photoreceptor membrane channels, and spontaneous neurotransmitter release.

The second part of the tvi curve is called the Square Root Law or (de Vries-Rose Law) region. This part of the curve is limited by quantal fluctuation in the background. Rose (3) proposed that the visual threshold would be quantal limited. The visual system is usually compared with a theoretical construct, an ideal light detector. An ideal detector can detect and encode each absorbed quantum of light and is limited only by the noise due to quantal fluctuations in the source. To detect the stimulus, the stimulus must be sufficiently exceed the fluctuations of the background (noise).

Because the variability in quanta increases with the number of quanta absorbed, threshold would increase with background luminance. In fact, the increase in threshold should be proportional to the square root of the background luminance, hence, the slope of 0.5 in a log-log plot. For the rod pathway, a slope of 0.6 is often found (4). Barlow (5) explored the conditions that influenced the transition from the Square Root Law to Weber's Law (see below). He concluded that for brief, small test spots, increment thresholds rise as the square root of the background over the entire photopic range. Spots of large areas and long durations have slopes close to Weber's Law. Other spatio-temporal configurations result in different proportions of each region.

When plotted using log ΔL versus log L coordinates, the Weber law section ideally has a slope 1. For the rod pathway, a slope 0.8 or less is found, implying that the rod pathway does operate under true Weber conditions. This section of the curve demonstrates an important aspect of our visual system. Our visual system is designed to distinguish objects from its background. In the real world, objects have contrast, which is constant and independent of ambient luminance. Therefore, the principle of Weber's law can be applied to contrast that remains constant regardless of illumination changes. This is called contrast constancy or contrast invariance, with this contrast level defined as Weber's constant. Contrast constancy can be mathematically expressed as ΔL /L = constant. ΔL is the increment threshold on a background L. The constant is also known as the Weber constant or Weber fraction. The Weber constant for the rod and cone is 0.14 (6) and 0.02 to 0.03 (7), respectively. Within the cone pathways, the S-cone pathway again has different characteristics to those of the longer-wavelength pathway with a Weber constant of around 0.09 (8).

Section 4 of the curve (Fig. 12) shows rod saturation at high background luminance. The slope begins to increase rapidly, and the rod system starts to become unable to detect the stimulus. This section of the curve occurs for the cone mechanism under high background light levels.

About the Authors

Image psych1fu1.jpg
Michael Kalloniatis was born in Athens Greece in 1958. He received his optometry degree and Master's degree from the University of Melbourne. His PhD was awarded from the University of Houston, College of Optometry, for studies investigating colour vision processing in the monkey visual system. Post-doctoral training continued at the University of Texas in Houston with Dr Robert Marc. It was during this period that he developed a keen interest in retinal neurochemistry, but he also maintains an active research laboratory in visual psychophysics focussing on colour vision and visual adaptation. He was a faculty member of the Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences at the University of Melbourne until his recent move to New Zealand. Dr. Kalloniatis is now the Robert G. Leitl Professor of Optometry, Department of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Auckland. e-mail: ua.ude.wsnu@sitainollak.m

Image psych1fu2.jpg
Charles Luu was born in Can Tho, Vietnam in 1974. He was educated in Melbourne and received his optometry degree from the University of Melbourne in 1996 and proceeded to undertake a clinical residency within the Victorian College of Optometry. During this period, he completed post-graduate training and was awarded the post-graduate diploma in clinical optometry. His areas of expertise include low vision and contact lenses. During his tenure as a staff optometrist, he undertook teaching of optometry students as well as putting together the "Cyclopean Eye", in collaboration with Dr Michael Kalloniatis. The Cyclopean Eye is a Web based interactive unit used in undergraduate teaching of vision science to optometry students. He is currently in private optometric practice as well as a visiting clinician within the Department of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Melbourne.

References

1.
Aubert H. Physiologie der Netzhaut. Breslau: E. Morgenstern; 1865. (Ger).
2.
Aguilar M, Stiles WS. Saturation of the rod mechanism of the reina at high levels of stimulation. Opt Acta (Lond). 1954;1:59–65.
3.
Rose A. The sensitivity performance of the human eye on a absolute scale. J Opt Soc Am. 1948;38:196–208. [PubMed: 18901781]
4.
Hallett PE. The variations in visual threshold measurement. J Physiol. 1969;202:403–419. [PMC free article: PMC1351489] [PubMed: 5784294]
5.
Barlow HB. Increment thresholds at low intensities considered as signal noise discriminations. J Physiol. 1958;141:337–350. [PMC free article: PMC1358868] [PubMed: 13429514]
6.
Cornsweet TN. Visual perception. New York: Academic Press; 1970.
7.
H Davson. Physiology of the eye. 5th ed. London: Macmillan Academic and Professional Ltd.; 1990.
8.
Stiles WS. Colour vision: the approach through increment threshold sensitivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1959;75:100–114.
9.
Pirenne MH. Dark adaptation and night vision. In: Davson H, editor. The eye. Vol 2. London: Academic Press; 1962.
10.
Bartlett NR. Dark and light adaptation. In: Graham CH, editor. Vision and visual perception. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.; 1965.
11.
Osterberg G. Topography of the layer of rods and cones in the human retina. Acta Ophthalmol Suppl. 1935;6:1–103.
Copyright: © 2024 Webvision .

All copyright for chapters belongs to the individual authors who created them. However, for non-commercial, academic purposes, images and content from the chapters portion of Webvision may be used with a non-exclusive rights under a Attribution, Noncommercial 4.0 International (CC BY-NC) Creative Commons license. Cite Webvision, http://webvision.med.utah.edu/ as the source. Commercial applications need to obtain license permission from the administrator of Webvision and are generally declined unless the copyright owner can/wants to donate or license material. Use online should be accompanied by a link back to the original source of the material. All imagery or content associated with blog posts belong to the authors of said posts, except where otherwise noted.

Bookshelf ID: NBK11525PMID: 21413386

Views

Related Items in Bookshelf

Related information

  • PMC
    PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed
    Links to PubMed

Similar articles in PubMed

See reviews...See all...

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...