U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine; Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education; Board on Children, Youth, and Families; Committee on Exploring the Opportunity Gap for Young Children from Birth to Age Eight; Hutton R, Allen LR, editors. Closing the Opportunity Gap for Young Children. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2023 Oct 2.

Cover of Closing the Opportunity Gap for Young Children

Closing the Opportunity Gap for Young Children.

Show details

3Opportunity Gaps in the Education Experienced by Children in Grades K–3

In this chapter, we continue our discussion of opportunity gaps in education and our analysis of outcomes for students in the context of historical structural drivers that create disparities for young children in the early grades (see Chapter 1 for a broader discussion of historical structural drivers). This chapter reviews the evidence related to these drivers and their effects on student outcomes in grades K–3. We also discuss barriers to access to high-quality education and other supports that can benefit young children and their families, and the differential experiences that children and families may experience in accessing these supports.

Similar to the discussion in Chapter 2, the focus of this chapter is on examining evidence related to gaps in access experienced by children and their families, disparities in quality experiences during the early grades, and the ways in which past and present structural drivers can perpetuate this inequity. We also highlight promising policies, practices, and programs with the potential to close the opportunity gap for children in grades K–3. The review of evidence presented in this chapter informed the committee's recommendations, presented in Chapter 8, for increasing access to equitable and high-quality learning, as well as creating more inclusive quality frameworks.

High-quality early care and education (ECE) followed by quality, well-funded, early elementary education is associated with a host of positive outcomes for children in the early grades, including and especially those who have historically been marginalized (Johnson & Jackson, 2018). Unfortunately, systemic factors in the early elementary grades can sustain and amplify many of the same disparities in opportunities and outcomes that begin earlier in children's educational trajectories. Indeed, recent literature focused on long-term developmental outcomes for young children, in particular those growing up in contexts characterized by lack of access to resources and supportive health and educational services, reexamines classic studies, such as those of the Abecedarian and HighScope Perry Preschool Project and the Head Start Impact Study, finding some evidence of “fade-out” in the elementary school years (Puma et al., 2010; Durkin et al., 2022). By contrast, two other recent meta-analyses looking at the medium- and long-term effects of ECE found that it is beneficial in promoting child well-being and lowering longer-term education costs (McCoy et al., 2017), and that high-quality, well-implemented preschool programs can increase early learning gains that have lasting effects through later years of schooling (Meloy, Gardner, & Darling-Hammond, 2019). Another recent study found evidence of an association between attending high-quality ECE and continued positive outcomes in early academic skills through grade 3 (Horm et al., 2022). Horm and colleagues (2022) note the need for more research to study the mechanisms that help sustain early gains or can cause fade-out in the early grades.

The funding structure for K–12 education relies heavily on local funding, and in many cases, federal and state funding does not adequately compensate for funding gaps at the local level. Research shows that these funding gaps, in combination with policies that have disproportionate negative effects on children from racialized1 and marginalized backgrounds and interpersonal biases among adults who work with children, result in unequal experiences for young children from racialized backgrounds, those in low-income communities, those who speak a language other than English, and those with disabilities. Further, the misalignment between the ECE and early elementary systems in their definitions and expectations of quality disrupts continuity in gains experienced by young children and further perpetuates opportunity gaps.

The National Academies study Transforming the Workforce for Children from Birth through Age 8 stresses the importance of continuity across the birth to 8 spectrum—both in the systems in which the education workforce works and in positive, high-quality experiences and environments (Institute of Medicine and National Research Council [IOM & NRC], 2015). The report focuses on two dimensions of continuity: (1) vertical continuity of high-quality experiences across diverse education settings and (2) alignment of learning expectations, curricula, instructional strategies, assessments, and learning environments. The report emphasizes that these dimensions of continuity should be based on evidence on child development and be informed by evidence-based best practices. The report also notes that continuity also includes coordinated services and policies that can affect children in this age range and communication among providers, including educators, health care providers and services, mental health professionals, social services, and other community support agencies. They conclude that coordination and collaboration cannot be achieved without removing systemic barriers and improving supports to achieve better communication and interaction among providers and across settings (IOM & NRC, 2015).

CURRENT POLICY, FUNDING, AND SYSTEMS FOR EARLY ELEMENTARY EDUCATION

In the United States, state governments are obligated to provide public education to all school-aged children. Nonetheless, opportunity gaps exist within this system. Funding disparities in K–12 education affect access to well-resourced and quality programs (Lloyd & Harwin, 2021). As with ECE, these disparities impact a disproportionate number of students of color, although there is considerable variability in this regard across and within states (Raikes & Darling-Hammond, 2019). To illustrate, neighboring suburban counties outspend Chicago by more than $10,000 per student (Raikes & Darling-Hammond, 2019).

A key factor shaping funding inequities is the prevailing school funding model that relies on local property taxes. Thus, children who live in low-income neighborhoods are more likely to attend underresourced schools (Raikes & Darling-Hammond, 2019). Funding disparities in school construction and modernization are also shaped by property wealth. Districts with high property wealth—which serve predominantly White learners—spend significantly more on school construction and modernization compared with low-income districts (Brunner, Schwegman, & Vincent, 2022). Furthermore, district size and racial makeup mediate funding patterns. For instance, small school districts serving mainly White students receive $23 billion more than districts serving minority majority districts (EdBuild, 2019). The Education Trust reports that districts educating mainly White students receive $1,800 more per student per year compared with districts serving primarily students of color (Latino, African American, Native American; Morgan & Amerikaner, 2018).

Federal funding for special populations, such as children in low-income communities, English learners, and children with disabilities, is generally insufficient to bridge state and local gaps, largely because these federal funding streams are underfunded. Research indicates that funding gaps exist nationally between White and Black, White and Latino, and higher-income and lower-income students (Shores, Lee, & Williams, 2021). Black students receive about $400 less than White students, while lower-income students receive about $430 less than higher-income students. The largest gap is between White and Latino students, with Latino students receiving about $1,200 less than their White peers. Shores, Lee, and Williams (2021) examined these gaps in per pupil spending at the national, state, and district levels. They found the largest gaps nationally, explained by differences in education spending across states and the distribution of students of color and lower-income students in states that invest less in education. For example, at the state level, Shores, Lee, and Williams found higher per pupil expenditures for Black, Hispanic, and lower-income students than for White and higher-income students. At the district level, more funding is generally allocated to Black, Hispanic, and lower-income students, with the gap between Hispanic and White students being largest. At the national level, however, resource distribution was found to be more regressive, with Black, Hispanic, and lower-income students receiving lower per pupil spending and lower capital expenditures (Shores, Lee, & Williams, 2021).

Probing further the disparities across states, Baker (2017) reports a national perspective on school funding inequalities. His main findings include the following:

  • School funding levels continue to be characterized by wide disparities among states, ranging from a high of $18,165 per pupil in New York to a low of $5,838 in Idaho when adjusted for regional differences.
  • Many of the lowest-funding states, such as Arizona, Idaho, Nevada, North Carolina, and Texas, allocate a very low percentage of their states' economic capacity to funding for public education.
  • Twenty‐one states are regressive, providing less funding to school districts with higher concentrations of low‐income students.
  • Only a handful of states—Delaware, Minnesota, New Jersey, and Massachusetts—have generally high funding levels and also provide significantly more funding to districts where student poverty is highest.
  • Low rankings on school funding fairness correlate with poor state performance on key resource indicators, including less access to ECE, noncompetitive wages for teachers, and higher teacher:student ratios.

In light of such funding differences, a research question consistently raised in the literature is whether school spending matters. The available evidence offers an affirmative answer to this question. Increased school funding is associated with better academic performance, higher graduation rates, and improved income in adult life (Jackson, 2018), with the most pronounced effects seen in children from low-income households (Jackson, Johnson, & Persico, 2016). Lafortune, Rothstein, and Schanzenbach (2018) studied the impacts of school finance reforms on student achievement and found that the impacts of increased funding for low-income school districts were immediate, strong, and sustainable. Of significance, Lafortune, Rothstein, and Schanzenbach (2018) found that a one-time $1,000 increment in per student annual spending had a relative achievement impact over a 10-year period of “between 0.12 and 0.24 standard deviations” in low-income districts (p. 6). These researchers also found that funding reforms were effective in reducing inequities across districts, although “other policy tools aimed at closing within-district achievement gaps will be needed to address overall equity concerns” (Lafortune, Rothstein, & Schanzenbach, 2018, p. 4 [emphasis in original]), including achievement gaps between students of color and White learners and between high- and low-income groups.

Jackson (2018) conducted a comprehensive review of the research on school spending and student outcomes. The review distinguished between older studies categorized largely as descriptive and recent research aiming to draw causal inferences. A consistent finding across the two kinds of studies was a positive association between increased school spending and learner outcomes. This was “true across studies that use different datasets, examine different time periods, rely on different sources of variation, and employ different statistical techniques” (Jackson, 2018, p. 13). Nevertheless, Jackson cautions about potential contextual effects not yet well understood. For instance, some research on capital construction and Title I spending does not consistently support the link between school funding and learner outcomes. Critically, however, infrastructure and facility investments may have important effects on children beyond academics—for example, in health and safety.

Jackson, Johnson, and Persico (2016) studied the effects of school reform efforts and found that “a 10% increase in per pupil spending each year for all twelve years of public school leads to 0.27 more completed years of education, 7.25% higher wages, and a 3.67 percentage-point reduction in the annual incidence of adult poverty; effects are much more pronounced for children from low-income families” (p. 1). The authors estimate that the effect of a permanent increase in per pupil spending throughout all school years of about 22.7% (about $2,800 in per pupil spending) for low-income learners would eliminate the achievement gap between high- and low-income students. Three states stand out for instituting school spending reforms that have produced noteworthy improvements in student outcomes and achievement gaps (Baker, 2017). Massachusetts, New Jersey, and Minnesota instituted reforms that included increasing funding for school districts serving a sizable number of marginalized learners; expanding enrollment in quality preschool and investing in school readiness programs and Head Start; and strengthening professional capacity and development through measures that included salary increases, higher professional standards, and sustained professional development (Baker, 2017).

School spending also matters for students with disabilities. Cruz and colleagues (2020) report a positive association between greater spending on special education programs and growth in the number of students with disabilities meeting or exceeding standards for English language arts. However, this pattern was not observed in high-poverty schools, which generally had fewer certified teachers compared with low-poverty schools, suggesting that funding and qualified teachers are both critical. It is important to note that the association between increases in spending on special education programs and growth in the number of students meeting or exceeding English language arts standards also benefited learners without disabilities in both high- and low-poverty schools (Cruz et al., 2020). Taken together, these findings indicate that school spending reforms are linked to improved academic outcomes for students with and without disabilities (with some important contextual caveats).

Access to and Funding for Out-of-School Time

Access barriers extend into what is traditionally labeled as “after school” and commonly referred to in the youth development sector as out-of-school-time (OST) programming. The trajectory of OST programming is similar to that of ECE programming—born out of labor market shifts and societal needs, OST programs are often underresourced despite the clear evidence of their positive impact on child development gains (Mahoney, Parente, & Zigler, 2009). ECE and OST programs also share a common history of disinvestment and inequity—specifically across communities of color and low-income communities—which manifests in barriers to access, funding, and quality. Families can receive assistance in paying for and accessing school-age care through only two funding streams: the Child Care and Development Block Grant (CCDBG) and the 21st Century Community Learning Centers initiative. CCDBG is the main federal funding source for helping families afford child care, including OST care and care for school-aged children. Although school-age child care is a large part of the child care subsidy system, it is often forgotten in policy and systems conversations. In fact 44% of CCDBG participants are school-aged children between the ages of 5 and 13 (Afterschool Alliance, n.d.). The 21st Century Community Learning Centers program, created in 1994 by Congress, provides grant funding for the creation of community learning centers with the goal of increasing access to academic enrichment opportunities after school and during the summer months for children—in particular, those from lower-performing schools or schools where there is high poverty (Department of Education, 2023).

In 2020, an all-time high of 87% of parents supported public funding for after-school programs (Fortner, Hardy, & Schmit, 2021). According to the Afterschool Alliance, the most prominent barriers families faced in accessing these programs were availability, cost, and the safety of children commuting to and from a program. After-school programs are in limited supply: for every child enrolled in such programs, three are waiting to enroll, suggesting that 24.6 million children might participate in after-school care if it were available. The access issue is even more marked for Black and Latino children in families with low incomes. A survey from the Afterschool Alliance found that, if given the opportunity, 58% of Black children and 55% of Latino children, compared with 46% of White children, would enroll in school-age after-school programs. In rural communities, more than 4.5 million children who are not in OST programming would be if a program were available to them—a 43% increase since 2014; 52% of respondents in rural communities were families with lower incomes. Note that these data were not disaggregated intersectionally by race and income (Afterschool Alliance, 2021, 2022).

As for the workforce, OST providers are paid lower wages and receive fewer benefits compared with other school-age care providers because their positions are often part-time and generally require fewer credentials. The estimated cost to reach all eligible school-age children through CCDBG ranges from $48.4 billion to $79.6 billion, taking into account such variables as increased market-rate payments to states that would go toward higher wages (Fortner, Hardy, & Schmit, 2021).

Regardless of the tremendous need, overall access to OST programming has increased over the last few decades alongside specific program offerings within OST, such as health and wellness programs; science, technology, engineering, and math programs; arts-based programs; and social-emotional learning programs. Yet despite the increase in program offerings overall, the disparity in OST participation between students from wealthy and low-income households has increased (Gardner, Roth, & Brooks-Gunn, 2009). Across demographics, geography, and income, access barriers are consistently increasing as families report challenges related to cost, children having safe transport from school to the program location, lack of available program offerings in the community, and inconvenient program locations. Black (59%) and Latino (56%) families living in rural, predominantly low-income communities report not having a safe way for their children to get from school to the after-school program as a primary barrier to enrollment (Afterschool Alliance, 2022).

Again, consistent with ECE programs, OST after-school programs have seen a surge in demand as the field has shown undeniable evidence of opportunities for positive impacts on children (Lehrer-Small, 2021). However, limited funding, lack of access to and availability of OST programs, and an underpaid workforce continue to demonstrate the pervasiveness of the opportunity gaps children experience as they move along the developmental continuum.

Special Education

Later in childhood, compared with the early years, the percentage of children who qualify for and receive special education almost doubles. In 2019, among children aged 6–21, 6,374,498, or 9.7% of the resident population in that age range, were served in 49 states, the District of Columbia, and Bureau of Indian Education schools. The most common disability categories among children in this age group served under the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) were specific learning disability (37.1%), other health impairment (16.8%), speech or language impairment (16.3%), autism (11.0%), “other disabilities combined” (7%), intellectual disability (6.5%), and emotional disturbance (5.4%). In almost every category, children of color, including Black, Latino, American Indian/Alaska Native, and Hawaiian and other Pacific Islander, were overrepresented in the special education system, generally in the categories of intellectual disabilities, learning disabilities, and emotional/behavioral disorder (NRC, 2002; National Academies, 2019), whereas White and Asian American children were underrepresented in these categories. As elaborated in Table 3-1, representation patterns vary by disability category and racial/ethnic/language group. Cruz and Firestone (2022) conducted a study in a large urban school district in California to trace the timing of special education identification. Their findings indicate that African American and Hispanic/Latino students tended to be identified in later grades (after K–6) and in disability categories associated with greater levels of segregation.

TABLE 3-1. Risk Ratios for Students with Disabilities Aged 5–21 (served by Individuals with Disabilities Education Act [IDEA] Part B) by Racial/Ethnic Group and Disability Category, Fall 2019.

TABLE 3-1

Risk Ratios for Students with Disabilities Aged 5–21 (served by Individuals with Disabilities Education Act [IDEA] Part B) by Racial/Ethnic Group and Disability Category, Fall 2019.

Segregated Learning

In the K–12 system, data on segregated learning among students with disabilities are collected according to the percentage of time children spend in the general education classroom—less than 40%, 40–80%, or more than 80%. According to data from the Department of Education (2021b) for the 2019 school year, while most school-aged students served by IDEA (64.8%) spent 80% or more of their time inside a general education classroom, this figure varied by state, disability type, and racial/ethnic group. Alabama had the highest percentage of children with disabilities who spent 80% or more of the school day alongside their peers without disabilities inside the regular classroom, while New Jersey had the lowest percentage. With respect to disability type, children with intellectual disabilities or multiple disabilities were the least likely and children with speech impairments or learning disabilities were the most likely to spend time in general education settings.

There were also differences by race. White children were the most likely to spend most of the day in general education settings compared with children of all other races and ethnicities (Figure 3-1). This evidence indicates gaps in access to inclusive learning opportunities between White children and their peers from other racial/ethnic groups (Fierros & Conroy, 2002; Skiba et al., 2006). Similar patterns have been documented in neighborhood and charter schools (Waitoller & Maggin, 2020).

FIGURE 3-1. Percentage of students aged 5–21 served under the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) Part B within racial/ethnic groups, by educational environment, fall 2019.

FIGURE 3-1

Percentage of students aged 5–21 served under the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) Part B within racial/ethnic groups, by educational environment, fall 2019. 1Percentage of day spent inside the regular class is defined as the (more...)

The disproportionate representation of students of color in special education in general and in segregated settings in particular reflects the complex links between race and disability. These disparities are most noticeable in disability categories—such as learning disabilities, emotional disturbance, and mild intellectual disability—considered most subjective because of the greater role played by professional judgment in diagnostic decisions. Grindal et al. (2019) used individual-level data from three states to analyze racial disparities in special education. They documented greater racial disparities in these more “subjective” disability categories relative to disabilities typically diagnosed in the health care system (e.g., deafness, visual impairment). These authors also found that African American and Latino students were placed in more segregated settings compared with their White counterparts, irrespective of income level. Another study using individual-level data from a large school district and relying on a longitudinal design covering a decade (Cooc, 2022) found that all students with disabilities experienced decreasing levels of inclusion in general education as they became older. Nonetheless, African American learners were the most affected (after controlling for disability type), while Asian American/Pacific Islander students were more included compared with their White and Latino peers. It is important to note that a key challenge in understanding the complex, often ambiguous, and even contradictory findings from studies on racial disparities in special education and disability segregation is the absence of clear theoretical frameworks underlying this knowledge base (Artiles, 2011; Ahram, Voulgarides, & Cruz, 2021).

Indeed, there is a long-standing concern regarding the disproportionate under- and overidentification of learners of color in disability categories. Two National Academies reports addressing this concern were released 20 years apart (NRC, 1982, 2002), and scholarly debates on the issue continue to unfold (Morgan et al., 2015; Skiba et al., 2016). Both patterns can be problematic and can perpetuate opportunity gaps. Underidentification is a problem if children who need services are not diagnosed so that they receive supports. In contrast, overidentification is problematic if a diagnosis is the result of opportunity gaps, false positives, biases, or the conflation of cultural or linguistic differences with disability.

Disproportionality in identification of learners for special education is a complex phenomenon that takes different forms depending on the level of the system in question (national, state, regional, city, or district); the disability category under consideration; the age and grade level of children and youth; the racial and linguistic backgrounds of students; and the role of contextual and ideological factors, including racism and discrimination (NRC, 1982, 2002; Artiles & Trent, 1994; Skiba et al., 2008; Artiles, 2011; Sullivan & Artiles, 2011; Harry & Klingner, 2014; Cruz & Rodl, 2018; Frederick & Shifrer, 2018; Fish, 2019). Methodological and theoretical considerations shape (often conflicting) findings about disproportionality (Waitoller, Artiles, & Cheney, 2010; Cruz & Rodl, 2018). A complex debate has persisted around the role of race and social class. Some researchers argue that the overrepresentation of learners of color in special education is due to their high levels of poverty, whereas others suggest that race plays a critical role (Skiba et al., 2008; Artiles, 2019). Research findings on this issue have been mixed, and again, methodological factors could explain some of these ambiguous patterns. In a recent review of this literature, Cruz and Rodl (2018, p. 10) conclude that:

the ways in which each study conceptualized SES [socioeconomic status] varied, and, thus, results varied. When studies used aggregated measures for SES, they tended to report overrepresentation of students from racially and ethnically diverse backgrounds in more affluent areas. When considering SES by disaggregated free and reduced lunch measures, results were mixed. When using more specific continuous or composite indicators, much of the variability in special education identification could be attributed to SES.

Other studies with an explicit focus on contextual and cultural historical influences (e.g., history of race relations and racial segregation in the school and community, staff beliefs about race, and deficit views of communities of color) have documented the key role of race in overrepresentation at the district and school levels (Eitle & Eitle, 2002; Skiba et al., 2008; Kramarczuk Voulgarides et al., 2021; Tefera, Siegel-Hawley, & Sjogren, 2022).

The evidence suggests that children of color and those living in rural areas tend to be diagnosed with disabilities later than their White peers of similar age (Barnard-Brak et al., 2021), a problem that creates opportunity gaps in light of the importance of intervening early to provide services for these children (National Academies, 2017). At the same time, there is evidence that some groups of children (e.g., African Americans, Native Americans, English learners) tend to be overidentified in certain categories at the national or sometimes at the regional or state level (Skiba et al., 2008). The issue is most stark in the kinds of more “subjectively” diagnosed disability categories noted earlier, such as intellectual disability, learning disabilities, and emotional disturbance—categories that not only require greater professional (subjective) interpretation in the identification process but also tend to be characterized by overrepresentation of Black and Native American children and placement of learners in segregated settings relative to children in other disability categories. For instance, Cruz and Firestone (2022) found that African American and Latino students were identified in later grades relative to White students in the categories of emotional/behavioral disorders and intellectual disabilities, which tend to be served in more segregated settings compared with other disability categories. Table 3-2 presents the odds ratios for special education identification by grade.

TABLE 3-2. Odds Ratios for Special Education Identification by Grade Level and Student Group.

TABLE 3-2

Odds Ratios for Special Education Identification by Grade Level and Student Group.

Identification Rates and Placement

Cruz and Firestone (2022) also found that parent education level was positively associated with low-stigma diagnoses (e.g., speech and language impairments, autism). They frame this finding as an instance of opportunity hoarding exercised by privileged parents, given that different disability categories are associated with more or less stigma and services that require more or fewer resources. They explain that “for most disability categories, higher parent education levels were associated with decreased odds of placement; however, for the autism category, as parent education level increased, odds ratios also increased” (Cruz & Firestone, 2022, p. 108; see also Ong-Dean, 2009; Shifrer, 2013).

Disentangling disability from typical language learning for children who speak a language other than English at home has also resulted in under- or overdiagnosis among this group, depending on the context and level of analysis (Castro & Artiles, 2021). English learners tend to be underidentified at the national level in low-incidence categories such as severe autism, moderate/severe intellectual disabilities, and severe emotional/behavioral disorders (National Academies, 2017). Differences in identification rates are shaped by gaps in access related to a host of structural and technical factors, such as variability in referral rates that may be mediated by professionals' misunderstanding of the intersections between speaking multiple languages and disabilities (including myths about second language development), linguistic and cultural barriers in accessing services and service provision, parents' understanding and navigation of health and education service systems, and access to insurance, among other factors (National Academies, 2017). To illustrate, professionals can incorrectly associate English learners' behaviors associated with acquiring a second language with learning disorders, but may hesitate to refer these learners to special education because of this ambiguity (Klingner et al., 2005; Artiles, Klingner, & Tate, 2006). Table 3-3 outlines similarities in behaviors associated with second language acquisition and learning disability that can potentially lead to incorrect referrals to special education for English learners.

TABLE 3-3. Behaviors Associated with Learning Disabilities and Second Language Acquisition.

TABLE 3-3

Behaviors Associated with Learning Disabilities and Second Language Acquisition.

Researchers have documented underidentification of English learners in grades K–12 in urban school districts in California compared with English-proficient and White students (Artiles et al., 2005). When the data were disaggregated by grade, however, overrepresentation patterns were noticeable; specifically, this was the case in middle and high school, with particularly large gaps in 12th grade, when English learners were more than three times as likely to be placed in special education compared with their English-proficient counterparts. These districts categorized English learners in two subgroups—those with limited proficiency in English only and those with limited proficiency in both the primary language and English. The latter group represents a theoretically controversial category since it implies that these students are not proficient in any language; the group is described as semilingual or theorized as being languageless in the literature (Rosa, 2016). Of note, this latter group had a substantially higher probability of being identified in the learning disability, language impairment, and intellectual disability categories relative to the other three groups (English learners with limited English proficiency, English-proficient learners, and White learners; Artiles et al., 2005).

IDEA includes provisions on monitoring disproportionality. States have been required to report on disproportionate patterns since 1997, and since 2004 have been expected to create and implement policies and procedures to prevent or remedy inappropriate overidentification or disproportionate representation of learners of color. States cited for disproportionality can use up to 15% of IDEA funds to revise their procedures and eliminate this problem in general education. Unfortunately, because of the lack of clear federal guidance, “a sizable number of states have been able to meet the IDEA mandate by increasing their risk ratio and N criterion without addressing the problem of disproportionality” (Cavendish, Artiles, & Harry, 2014, p. 36; see also Albrecht et al., 2012).

Policies and Practices That Can Create Opportunity Gaps

A number of policy and practice issues create disparities in opportunity for children with disabilities, disproportionately affecting those of color and those in low-income households. Among others, these issues include less or later access to services, a lack of cultural and linguistically responsive services, and greater segregation in learning settings. These issues are in turn undergirded, in large part, by chronic underfunding of IDEA services. As mentioned in Chapter 3, the federal government has never fully funded its share of IDEA, leaving an outsized burden on state and local governments and families. This underfunding has influenced how many children are served by the program and has resulted in long waits for each step of the process, including screening, evaluation, eligibility determination, and service receipt; dosages of services that are often far less than what children need; variations in the quality of services children receive; and lack of coordination with other systems, such as child care.

Absenteeism

Children's learning opportunities may be truncated by everyday experiences that may impact their attendance, whether in ECE programs or in grades K–3. Chronic absenteeism is commonly defined as missing 10% or more of days in the school year (Chang, Bauer, & Byrnes, 2018) or more than 15 days (Department of Education, 2019). Research has shown the negative effects of absenteeism on school performance, achievement, and behavior in the early years and the pattern of absenteeism that continues into the elementary and later grades, as well as higher odds of retention, drop-out, and lifelong behaviors (Rhodes, Thomas, & Liles, 2018). In addition, chronic absenteeism has negative spillover effects on peers (Gottfried, 2009). National-level estimates of chronic absenteeism, most typically reported in the K–12 system, put it at about 10–16% (Chang & Romero, 2008; Department of Education, 2019). But these estimates mask significant geographic variation, with rates of preschool chronic absenteeism being higher in large cities (Gottfried, 2009)—for example, 50% in Newark (Chen & Rice, 2017), 36–45% in Chicago, 20–27% in Baltimore, and 35–37% in the District of Columbia (Connolly & Olson, 2012; Katz, Adams, & Johnson, 2015; Dubay & Holla, 2016; Ehrlich, Gwynne, & Allensworth, 2018). Note also that systematic reporting on attendance and chronic absenteeism has increased as a result of requirements under the Every Student Succeeds Act of 2015 (Katz, Adams, & Johnson, 2015).

Critical to understanding children's early learning opportunities is understanding the roots of absenteeism to the extent that they have been researched and the barriers that may impede high levels of participation. These include considerations related to transportation, hours of operation and program schedule, suspension, chronic illness or disability, housing instability, and community violence or insecurity, as well as misconceptions about the importance of attendance in the early years, among others (Dahlin & Squires, 2016; Chang, Bauer, & Byrnes, 2018; Humm Patnode, Gibbons, & Edmunds, 2018; Ramey & Ramey, 2019).

As a result of these barriers, differences in the incidence of absenteeism emerge across groups. A study conducted by the National Center for Children in Poverty (Romero & Lee, 2007) showed that children from families with income levels below 300% of the federal poverty threshold were four times more likely to have chronic absenteeism relative to children from families with incomes above that level. Findings have been similar for children in households receiving Temporary Assistance for Needy Families (Dubay & Holla, 2015). That study also found a higher incidence of chronic absenteeism in children from minoritized backgrounds and from households led by a single mother, with a high number of children, or with parents with lower educational attainment. Some research has found an association between feelings of discrimination and school absence, pointing to the potential role of racism and school climate in this problem (Bittencourt et al., 2009; Benner & Graham, 2011; Yang & Ham, 2017). Research has shown that maternal depression, substance abuse, homelessness, and mental illnesses of parents may also influence attendance (Gottfried, 2009; Dubay & Holla, 2015; Katz, Adams, & Johnson, 2015). Neighborhood and school factors, including neighborhood safety and the contribution of any tensions with the police and/or with social groups to a lack of safety for specific subgroups (Childs & Lofton, 2021) contribute to higher absenteeism as well (Fuhs, Nesbitt, & Jackson, 2018; Ansari & Pianta, 2019; Singer et al., 2021). Therefore, school infrastructure to support violence prevention, conflict resolution, and related measures is considered important to reducing chronic absenteeism (Kearney & Childs, 2021).

More generally, chronic absenteeism is a multifaceted issue strongly connected to community and poverty conditions, needing further study as well as multifaceted approaches (Childs & Lofton, 2021). Attending ECE programs has been shown to reduce later absenteeism (Gottfried, 2015; Ansari & Purtell, 2018), while initiatives to strengthen parent engagement (Smythe-Leistico & Page, 2018), to have nurses follow up with the families of chronically absent children (Kerr et al., 2011), and to serve breakfast after the start of the school day (Kirksey & Gottfried, 2021) have been found to be associated with increased attendance. A meta-analytical summary of behavioral, family, and academic interventions found small effects, with the authors suggesting that practices to improve attendance may be understudied (Eklund et al., 2022).

DIFFERENTIAL EXPERIENCES IN EARLY ELEMENTARY LEARNING SETTINGS

As in ECE systems, the quality and funding levels of the early elementary system profoundly impact children's experiences and outcomes. Although the term “quality” is used more commonly in discussing ECE, the concepts of structural quality2 and process quality3 (as discussed in Chapter 2) continue to be relevant in the early elementary grades, and deficits in both structural and process quality create gaps in opportunity for early elementary learners. Both structural and process quality and the interplay between them are influenced by systemic drivers, such as funding and policies. In communities with fewer resources, for example, larger class sizes, an element of structural quality, may result in fewer interactions between teachers and children—an element of process quality (Chaudry & Sandstrom, 2020).

As with the ECE system, there is no single or widely agreed-upon framework for quality in elementary school. Nonetheless, several common features have been studied over the years (Lowenstein et al., 2015; Ansari & Pianta, 2019). These include a combination of structural factors (poverty, racial and socioeconomic segregation); school-, district-, or state-level factors (e.g., funding, discipline policies, access to the curricula, ratios/class sizes, use of ability grouping in classrooms, organizational culture, community/family engagement); teacher-level factors (e.g., teacher turnover, teacher absences, distribution of well-qualified educators, educator bias); and child-level factors (e.g., academic outcomes, behavioral infractions; Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2005; Paro et al., 2009; Buttaro & Catsambis, 2019; Darling-Hammond, 2019; Papachristou et al., 2021). Research has examined a variety of teacher factors, for example, including licensure, stress and burnout, compensation, and efficacy, as well as domain-specific (e.g., literacy, math) instructional approaches.

While there is evidence for some factors and some locations (e.g., Montgomery County, Maryland), more work is needed on identifying equitable, holistic frameworks for high-quality elementary experiences that align with and promote continuity with high-quality ECE experiences (Brooks-Gunn, Markman-Pithers, & Rouse, 2016). Research has shown that quality transitions and alignment between ECE and elementary school are important for sustaining learning gains from the early years (Phillips, Austin, & Whitebook, 2016; Johnson & Jackson, 2018; Meloy, Gardner, & Darling-Hammond, 2019; Reynolds & Temple, 2019; Schweinhart, 2019). If quality is high in an ECE program but not in the early grades, it stands to reason that sustainment of ECE achievement gains will likely be low. Evidence also shows that exposure to high-quality processes across the early elementary years results in higher cognitive scores for learners (Vernon-Feagans et al., 2019).

Physical Infrastructure

The health and safety features of the physical buildings and spaces where children learn are perhaps the most foundational dimension of quality, yet research has uncovered profound inequities in this regard. Two Government Accountability Office reports on school infrastructure, separated by more than 20 years, document inequities by race and income (Government Accountability Office, 1996, 2020). Schools with higher proportions of children of color were more likely to cite concerns related to poor physical infrastructure; a recent finding was that low-poverty districts expended about $1 billion more on elementary school construction relative to high-poverty districts. The U.S. Commission on Civil Rights identified additional differences in facility quality by race, stating that schools with higher proportions of students of color were more likely to report poorer facility conditions and more temporary buildings (U.S. Commission on Civil Rights, 2018).

Ratios and Class Sizes

Low teacher:student ratios and smaller class sizes are associated with both developmental and academic gains; they are an important factor in ensuring that students are learning in a healthy and safe environment in which teachers are more likely to meet individual needs, including social-emotional needs. The literature shows that in classrooms with lower teacher:student ratios and smaller class sizes, less time is spent on behavior management, and students have less conflict in their interactions with peers. In addition, these classrooms are associated with higher-quality programming; positive student outcomes, such as greater receptive language and verbal initiative; richer teacher–child interactions; and higher rates of individualized attention (Ruopp, 1979; Barnett, Schulman, & Shore, 2004; Achilles, 2012). One study found that the quality of kindergarten classrooms was related to teacher:student ratios, as well as length of the school day (Paro et al., 2009). An experimental study found that elementary school students in small classes (13–17 students) outperformed their peers in large classes (22–26 students) on all tests, across every subject, in every grade (Finn, Pannozzo, & Achilles, 2003). And studies examining the relationship between elementary school class size and child outcomes in Wisconsin and California also yielded positive findings on academic outcomes, particularly for children from minoritized backgrounds (Molnar et al., 2000; Stecher & Bohrnstedt, 2000). It is important to note, however, that the effects of class size may be affected by other school factors such as high-quality classroom practices, administrative support, school infrastructure, and available space (Graue et al., 2007; Graue, Raucher, & Sherfinski, 2009).

More research is needed to examine the effects of teacher:student ratios and class size on children from various subgroups (e.g., children of color, English learners, children from low-income households) to better understand how these factors may affect opportunity gaps and perpetuate disparate outcomes. It is important to note that many factors come into play in examining ratios and class sizes, including the differences in ratios and class sizes being examined (e.g., 30 vs. 28 vs. 15 students); the availability of teachers and physical space to make it possible to decrease ratios and class sizes; and, critically, teacher quality.

Language of Instruction

Another important dimension of quality is the language of instruction. More than 11 million children, or about a third of all children under the age of 9, have a parent who speaks a language other than English at home (Migration Policy Institute, 2021). With appropriate supports, these children have the potential to become bilingual and biliterate. Bilingualism and biliteracy have been linked to a host of positive cognitive outcomes in the short term (Bialystok, 2017), academic and social outcomes in the medium term (e.g., National Academies, 2017; Steele et al., 2017), and economic and health outcomes in the long term (Callahan & Gándara, 2014; National Academies, 2017). In addition, research has found that a strong first language foundation facilitates language acquisition in subsequent languages. Indeed, children's academic skills and language proficiency in their second language is predicted by skills in their first language (Genesee et al., 2006; Sparks et al., 2008, 2009a, 2009b; August, Shanahan, & Escamilla, 2009). The strengths of bilingualism identified by research stand in stark contrast to the widely held perception that coming from a home where a language other than English is spoken is a deficit that must be remedied, as opposed to a strength to be fostered (Castro & Meek, 2022).

Studies have found that dual language immersion and similar bilingual learning approaches are associated with positive gains for children across a variety of academic and social-emotional domains (Genesee & Lindholm-Leary, 2013; National Academies, 2017). One review of data from 7.5 million student records in 36 school districts in 16 states found that high-quality, long-term bilingual programs closed the achievement gap between English learners and their peers after 5 to 6 years, while English-only and short-term transitional bilingual programs closed only about half of the gap (Collier & Thomas, 2017). Research from one large school district with a robust dual language program found that children—both English learners and those who spoke English at home—randomly assigned to dual language programs outperformed their peers in reading in fifth and eighth grades (Steele et al., 2017); no differences were noted in math and science. This study also found that English learners in dual language programs achieved English proficiency more rapidly relative to their peers in programs in which only English was used for instruction.

Harsh and Exclusionary Discipline Policies

Another structural dimension of quality that influences opportunity gaps is discipline policies and practices. In the 1990s and early 2000s, a zero tolerance approach to discipline took hold across the country, initially as a response to school safety concerns. These policies included mandatory suspensions or expulsions for students for specific infractions. Initially, those infractions included bringing weapons to school or making safety threats, but not long after, they expanded to include infractions unrelated to safety, including dress code violations, truancy, and developmentally appropriate tantrums in younger students. These policies were undergirded by the view that both minor and major disciplinary infractions should be punished harshly (Skiba & Knesting, 2001). Black children were disproportionately impacted by these policies in the form of higher rates of expulsion and suspension, resulting in an array of negative outcomes despite no credible evidence of worse behavior on the part of these children (Meek et al., 2020). Although such practices accelerated during this era, racial disparities in disciplinary practices have existed since schools began integrating after the Brown vs. Board of Education decision (Mills, 2016). In fact, data from the early 1970s indicate that the rate of suspension was at least twice as high for Black as for White children across the country (Kaeser, 1979).

Two decades later, these trends remain consistent. An analysis of Civil Rights Data Collection data (2015–2016 school year) for children in pre-K through elementary school found that Black children were disproportionately suspended and expelled in every state in the nation (Meek at al., 2020). The most recent wave of these data, from the 2017–2018 school year, revealed stubbornly consistent racial disparities in this regard between Black children and their peers. In addition, American Indian and Alaska Native children were 1.5 times as likely as their White peers to be suspended, while children with disabilities served under IDEA were 2.5 times as likely as their peers without disabilities to be expelled (Ryberg et al., 2021).

Federal data from the 2017–2018 school year revealed modest declines in exclusionary discipline in the elementary grades, especially compared with the more significant declines in rates for children in pre-K. In sum, exclusionary discipline in the elementary grades declined by 2% between the two most recent data collection periods; however, school-related arrests, expulsions with educational services, and referrals to law enforcement increased by 5%, 7%, and 12%, respectively (Department of Education, 2021a). Black children and boys in all racial/ethnic groups were consistently disciplined disproportionately across all discipline categories. Black girls in particular experienced substantial disproportionality in exclusionary discipline (Losen, 2017). One analysis found that Black girls were suspended at a rate four times greater than that of White girls. Black girls were disciplined disproportionately across every discipline category, including suspension, expulsion, referrals, school transfers due to behavior, and restraint (Epstein et al., 2020). Children with disabilities were disproportionately suspended and expelled compared with their peers without disabilities; these disparities were especially stark for Black children with disabilities, who experienced rates more than four times their share of enrollment. Although discipline rates have been inconsistent among Latino students, two recent studies using various methodological approaches identified higher rates of exclusionary discipline for these children compared with their White peers but lower rates compared with their Black peers (Morris & Perry, 2016; Owens & McLanahan, 2020; Gage et al., 2021).

Overall, it has been reported that statewide racial disparities in discipline mask deeper disparities across school districts and schools (Losen, 2017). Although IDEA requires monitoring and intervention on racial disparities in discipline for children with disabilities, only 20 states identified at least one school district with this problem; of the 10 states with the greatest racial disparities in discipline for this population, only four—Wisconsin, Connecticut, Texas, and North Carolina—reported a district with disproportionality patterns (Losen, 2017). See Table 3-4.

TABLE 3-4. Racial Disparities for Students with Disabilities (SWD) in Days of Lost Instruction per 100 Students Enrolled Due to Disciplinary Removal: Top 10 States (including the District of Columbia).

TABLE 3-4

Racial Disparities for Students with Disabilities (SWD) in Days of Lost Instruction per 100 Students Enrolled Due to Disciplinary Removal: Top 10 States (including the District of Columbia).

Suspensions and expulsions and disparities in these practices are fueled by a number of factors (Meek & Gilliam, 2016), including discipline policies (Skiba & Peterson, 2000), racial bias (Skiba et al., 2002, 2011; Okonofua & Eberhardt, 2015; Gilliam et al., 2016; Carter et al., 2017), lack of teacher preparation and support and poor working conditions (Gilliam & Shahar, 2006), unaddressed childhood trauma resulting from adverse experiences (Zeng et al., 2019), and school climate (McIntosh et al., 2021), among others. A recent study examining the Black–White discipline gap found that nearly half of this gap could be attributed to differential treatment and/or differential support offered to students, the largest single driver of the disparity (Owens & McLanahan, 2020). These authors also found that some of the gap could be attributed to the sorting of children into schools by race and ethnicity (21%), while differences in children's behavior accounted for the smallest amount of variance (Owens & McLanahan, 2020).

Virtually all studies examining differences in child behavior by race have relied on teacher or administrator ratings. Given the breadth of research on the influences of bias on perceptions of Black people, including young children, it is important to apply a critical lens to findings that rely on subjective perceptions of behavior. Indeed, research has found that racial disparities between Black and White students are driven largely by behaviors (e.g., disrespect, defiance) whose perception is subjective in nature, rather than those (e.g., vandalism, smoking) whose perception is objective in nature (Skiba et al., 2014). In addition, Losen (2017) found that school districts with high rates of suspension tended to apply disciplinary sanctions to students of color for minor infractions and nonviolent or nonthreatening behaviors, such as tardiness, loitering in the hall, dress code violations, truancy, profanity, carrying a cell phone, and smoking cigarettes. Research has provided evidence of racial bias in the perceptions of children's behavior and the discipline decisions made by adults in learning systems (see Marcelo & Yates, 2014; Okonofua & Eberhardt, 2015; Gilliam et al., 2016; Meek et al., 2020). A broader evidence base points to negative biases against Black individuals outside the classroom, including misperceiving Black boys as older than they are and less childlike than White boys, and more often mischaracterizing them as angry (Goff et al., 2014; Halberstadt et al., 2020). Likewise, Black girls are rated as more mature and needing less support, comforting, nurturing, and protection than White girls (Epstein et al., 2020). These biases also include empathy bias, whereby people as young as 7 years of age rate Black children as feeling less pain than White children (Goff et al., 2014). They include as well biases unveiled through automatic association tests in which respondents across racial groups more often associate Black versus White individuals with anger and aggression (Duncan, 1976; Hugenberg & Bodenhausen, 2003; Miller, Maner, & Becker, 2010). Collectively, these dimensions of bias influence how adults perceive behavior, whose behavior is scrutinized, and what decisions are made with respect to those perceptions, all of which can lead to disproportionality in exclusionary discipline.

These biases likely mediate responses to student behaviors, resulting in responses that have a negative impact on various outcomes. For instance, although harsher disciplinary sanctions for minor infractions may lead to a reduction in such behaviors, there is evidence that tough discipline models are associated with reduced high school graduation rates and increases in juvenile justice complaints (Sorensen et al., 2022). Principals with a tough discipline stance create conditions associated with lower academic achievement, wider Black–White achievement gaps, and reductions in student attendance (Sorensen et al., 2022). Indeed, research has shown a robust association between the Black–White discipline and achievement gaps after controlling for numerous factors (Pearman et al., 2019). Teacher behavior management approaches also have differential impacts on young children's perceived identities (e.g., “good,” “troublemaker”) and social relations (Gansen, 2021). Moreover, issues germane to race relations in broader societal contexts are linked to racial disparities in school discipline. For instance, Chin (2021) found that as schools decreased levels of racial segregation, racial disparities in discipline and special education identification increased. Similarly, racial disparities in school discipline were found to be associated with county-level indicators of racial bias (Riddle & Sinclair, 2019). And Perera (2021) documents how school districts with significant equity challenges (e.g., higher levels of racial segregation, substantial racial achievement gaps, larger proportion of racialized students) had a greater probability of receiving civil rights complaints.

Suspension and expulsion result in about 11 million days of missed school in one school year alone, breaking down disproportionately along gender and racial lines. Relative to other groups, Black children and boys in all racial/ethnic groups in one study missed more than twice as many days because of suspensions. Black boys missed the greatest number of days, and Black girls missed about twice as many days as their White female counterparts (Fabes et al., 2021). Losen (2017) reports that, at the national level, Black versus White children lost 76 more days of instruction. Another study found that exclusionary discipline accounted for one-fifth of academic disparities between Black and White children (Morris & Perry, 2016).

Unsurprisingly, suspension and expulsion are positively associated with grade retention, lower achievement, and dropping out of high school, and negatively associated with taking advanced math courses and attending college (Wald & Losen, 2003; Gregory, Skiba, & Noguera, 2010; Fabelo et al., 2011; Morris & Perry, 2016; Losen, 2017; Wolf & Kupchik, 2017; Mittleman, 2018; Pearman et al., 2019; Jabbari & Johnson, 2020)—all of which are outcomes in themselves, but also serve to further perpetuate opportunity gaps going forward. Research has found as well that exclusionary discipline does not reduce challenging behavior; rather, it is associated with increased “delinquent behavior,” and these increases are not moderated by race, indicating that these practices have similarly negative influences across groups (Gerlinger et al., 2021).

Since 2014, following the first federal publication of pre-K suspension and expulsion data, policies at the federal level have guided efforts to reduce exclusionary discipline in K–12 settings (Department of Justice & Department of Education, 2014). State and local legislative efforts to limit exclusionary discipline in the early grades, as well as executive branch efforts of state governors and agencies, have followed. These efforts include improving data collection, fostering family engagement, and providing professional development. Despite the number of new policies, however, these policies vary in both quality and content. During the Trump administration, the Obama-era K–12 federal discipline guidance was rolled back, but the effects on state and local policy are currently unclear. Under the Biden administration, federal officials are undertaking a review of the original policy and its subsequent repeal.

Data indicate that efforts aimed at simply reducing rates of exclusionary discipline have proven insufficient in addressing persistent disparities, pointing to the need to develop effective policies targeted specifically at closing the gaps among groups. Addressing this need has proven difficult, however, as researchers have examined statewide efforts to this end and found them to be ineffective at bridging the gaps (Linick, Garcia, & Grandpre, 2021; see also Cruz, Kulkarni, & Firestone, 2021, for a review of efforts to reduce disproportionality in exclusionary discipline; see Losen et al., 2015, for successful interventions to reduce discipline disparities).

Corporal punishment, defined as paddling, spanking, or other forms of physical punishment imposed on a child, is another form of harsh discipline that is used as a disciplinary practice in both pre-K and K–12 settings. The United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child, which the United States has signed but not ratified, states that no child should be subjected to “physical or mental violence, injury or abuse, neglect or negligent treatment, maltreatment or exploitation” at school or by a parent or legal guardian (United Nations, 1989); however, there is no federal law prohibiting corporal punishment, and it remains legal in public school settings in 19 states across the nation and in private school settings in all but 2 states. According to federal data, roughly 70,000 children across age groups, including more than 800 preschool-aged children, were subjected to corporal punishment during a given year (Department of Education, 2021c). As with exclusionary discipline, Black children, boys, and children with disabilities are disproportionately subjected to corporal punishment. Although only 11% of the nation's school districts practiced corporal punishment in 2013–2014, 10 Southern states accounted for 75% of corporal punishment, with Mississippi, Texas, Alaska, and Alabama accounting for more than 70% (Johnson, 2019)—38% of those suspended at least once and 37% of those corporally punished.

Restraint and seclusion are two additional practices used for disciplinary purposes, despite the fact that they were not intended for that purpose but to address emergencies in case of imminent harm to the child or others. The latest federal data show that 74,000 children in public K–12 settings were physically restrained over the course of a year, and 27,500 were subjected to seclusion—the practice of locking children in a room alone without the ability to get out. As with other forms of harsh discipline, restraint and seclusion are applied disproportionately to Black children and children with disabilities in particular. In K–12 settings, Black children make up 15% of total enrollment but 29% of those restrained and 23% of those secluded; children with disabilities represent 13% of total K–12 enrollment but 78% of those restrained and 77% of those secluded. As with exclusionary discipline and corporal punishment, there is no federal law prohibiting these practices, while a patchwork of state laws limit or place parameters around their use (Meek et al., 2020).

Research suggests that each of these discipline policies and practices is associated with adverse outcomes—academic, social, and psychological—as well as engagement with the criminal justice system later in life. Children who are excluded through suspension and expulsion are stigmatized and isolated (Rosenbaum, 2020) in addition to missing days of school and learning opportunities (Losen & Whitaker, 2018; Fabes et al., 2021). Related to these issues are findings indicating that children who are suspended or expelled have lower school engagement, are more likely to repeat a grade, and are less likely to graduate from high school (Browne, Losen, & Wald, 2001; Karega Rausch & Skiba, 2004; Arcia, 2006; Gregory, Skiba, & Noguera, 2010; Skiba, Arredondo, & Williams, 2014).

THE EDUCATION WORKFORCE IN GRADES K–3

The education workforce in grades K–3 has a profound effect on young children's experiences and outcomes. Three aspects of the workforce—the provision of supportive, enriching, and warm teacher–child relationships and interactions; teacher expectations and perceptions of behavior; and pedagogy, instruction, and access to enrichment—are discussed here.

Supportive, Enriching, and Warm Teacher–Child Relationships and Interactions

Children learn optimally in the context of warm and secure relationships with adults. Indeed, decades of research have revealed that adult–child attachment is predictive of a range of outcomes in children, including advanced cognitive and language development, academic achievement, and a range of social and emotional skills (Ewing & Taylor, 2009; Iruka, Burchinal, & Cai, 2010; IOM & NRC, 2015; Lee & Bierman, 2015; McCormick & O'Connor, 2015; Varghese, Vernon-Feagans, & Bratsch-Hines, 2019). Children typically form these attachments with their parents or primary caregivers, but can also form them with other adults in their lives, including ECE educators and teachers. Researchers have pointed to a number of processes that mediate the association between warm and secure attachments and child outcomes, including early confidence and competence at exploration, effective instruction and guidance, social competence with adults and peers, self-regulatory competence, and stress management (IOM & NRC, 2015).

Studies indicate that teacher–child relationships are associated with children's engagement and ability to optimize their learning experiences in the classroom. When children feel safe and secure in their relationships, they are more confident and better able to explore and engage actively in learning across all domains (Ladd & Burgess, 1999). These relationships form the foundation for the daily interactions that occur inside classroom or home learning environments, both positive and negative, and are associated with an array of factors that influence children's learning experiences. Positive critical teacher–child relationships, and all of their associated processes, are related to third-grade achievement and to children's perceptions of school, including feeling more positive about school and more excited about learning (Birch & Ladd, 1997; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2002; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004; O'Connor & McCartney, 2007). In turn, negative or conflictual teacher–child relationships have been found to predict challenges with peers (Palermo et al., 2007), as well as internalizing and externalizing behaviors (Roorda et al., 2011; Zatto & Hoglund, 2019). Importantly, research indicates that close and warm teacher–child relationships can play a protective role against discrimination faced by children of color (Redding, 2019). Some research has found that such teacher–child relationships may have a particularly large impact on the outcomes of children from lower-income households (Driscoll & Pianta, 2010).

Research has found that the quality of teacher–child relationships is associated with a number of child demographic characteristics, including gender, race/ethnicity, disability, and socioeconomic status, as well as teacher demographic characteristics. It is important to understand these findings in the context of the systems, policies, and funding decisions that undergird them. That is, it is not the child or teacher demographics per se that are driving these differences, but systemic factors associated with these demographic variables. These systemic factors include differential access to resources and funding, segregation patterns, and other factors previously discussed that influence bias at all levels, from organizational policies to interpersonal interactions (Downey & Pribesh, 2004; Frawley, 2005; Campbell, 2015). Together, these factors drive gaps in the quality of teacher–child relationships and daily interactions, which can contribute to opportunity gaps.

Girls compared with boys are typically rated as having more positive relationships with teachers (Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Palermo et al., 2007; Ewing & Taylor, 2009; Collins et al., 2017). Studies also indicate that girls versus boys may have more to gain from these positive relationships and face more significant consequences when their relationships with teachers are poor (Baker, 2006; Ewing & Taylor, 2009; Ly et al., 2012). One study found that girls having more conflictual relationships with their teachers showed lower levels of math achievement and math growth relative to boys with comparable relationships (McCormick & O'Connor, 2015).

In general, research has found that teachers report having less close and more conflictual relationships with Black and Latino children relative to White children (Hughes, 2005; Garner & Mahatmya, 2015; Goldberg & Iruka, 2022). Research also has found that teachers tend to have more negative perceptions of Black and Latino children (Tenenbaum & Ruck, 2007; Zimmermann, 2018). One study found that those children perceived by kindergarten teachers as visibly being from a racial/ethnic minority group in Canada were 50% less likely to report positive relationships with their teachers in fourth grade (Fitzpatrick et al., 2015). These findings accord with research showing that children's race/ethnicity is associated with various dimensions of adult–child interactions in learning settings (Dobbs & Arnold, 2009); for example, children of color generally receive less praise and positive attention compared with their White peers (see Tenenbaum & Ruck, 2007).

It is critical to interpret these findings in light of data indicating that Black and Latino children are much less likely than their White peers to have teachers who match their race/ethnicity and language background. Research findings with respect to teacher–child racial/ethnic match have been mixed, with some showing positive effects of the match on the learning experiences and academic outcomes of young children of color and others showing minimal measurable effects (Ho, Gol-Guven, & Bagnato, 2012; Garner, Shadur, & Toney, 2021). The issue of teacher–child racial/ethnic match may have particular importance for children with disabilities. One study found that for those children identified as most at risk for emotional or behavioral disorders, racial/ethnic match with their teacher appeared to matter more. That is, greater mismatch predicted teachers' perceived conflict with children, mediated by teachers' classroom management and self-efficacy (Kunemund et al., 2020). Another study found that the teacher–child relationship served as a protective factor for children with developmental delays or disabilities and provided significant advantages for their development compared with their peers with similar delays or disabilities who had less close relationships with their teachers (Baker, 2006). Overall, it is important to consider a number of complex variables, including the structural factors discussed above, language, culture, class, and the interrelationships among all those factors in examining the effects of teacher–child match and interpreting the mixed research findings on this issue to date.

Teacher Expectations and Perceptions of Behavior

Dimensions of the teacher–child relationship that may be less explicit or tangible in nature, such as teacher expectations and perceptions of behavior, can play a significant role in shaping children's experiences in learning systems. Research has identified a positive association between teacher–child relationships and teacher expectations (Trang & Hansen, 2021), and a large and robust research base indicates that teacher expectations are associated with an array of child outcomes, in some cases over and above children's previous performance (McKown & Weinstein, 2008; Harlin, Sirota, & Bailey, 2009; Hinnant et al., 2009; Sorhagen, 2013).

Research also shows, however, that teacher expectations are not always reflective of children's abilities. Black children are rated more negatively by White teachers than by Black teachers across an array of domains, including language, literacy, and behavior (Downer et al., 2016). White compared with Black teachers also have been found to have lower expectations for Black children (Saft & Pianta, 2001; Downey & Pribesh, 2004; Tenenbaum & Ruck, 2007; Murray, Murray, & Waas, 2008; Bates & Glick, 2013). Studies have shown as well that White compared with Black teachers are more likely to recommend exclusionary discipline and special education placement for Black children (Achilles, McLaughlin, & Croninger, 2007; Skiba et al., 2011; Wiley et al., 2013; Sullivan et al., 2014). Other research has found that teachers are more likely to think their classes are too difficult for Black and Latino students than for White students, even after controlling for test scores and homework completion (Cherng, 2017). One study examined teachers' expectations of Latino students' long-term trajectories, and found that most teachers predicted that these students would not go to college but instead would work in the service sector, with many attributing those outcomes to family-related rather than structural factors (Dabach et al., 2018). Research also has revealed differential expectations based on English learner status. One nationally representative study found that teachers had lower expectations for English learners than for learners with English as their first language, expectations that began in kindergarten and grew over time. Importantly, these differential expectations were not observed in bilingual or dual language schools (Umansky & Dumont, 2021). These findings may suggest that teachers' perceptions of children's behavior and abilities and the decisions they make on the basis of those perceptions are influenced by racial, language, and gender-based bias.

The findings of several studies support the role of child race in teachers' assumptions, perceptions, and discipline decisions. One study showed ECE professionals a video of four young children playing in a classroom setting—one Black girl, one Black boy, one White girl, and one White boy. The researchers used eye-tracking technology to examine where on the screen the ECE teachers were looking when asked to predict a challenging behavior. Results indicated that they spent significantly more time looking at the Black boy when anticipating challenging behavior, even though no challenging behavior was ever exhibited in the video (Gilliam et al., 2016). Boonstra (2021) used ethnographic methods to document how racialized and ableist school and classroom discourses constructed pathological identities for Black students and how those identities were in turn associated with surveillance practices that mediated behavioral escalations and physical restraint. This heightened level of scrutiny helps explain racial disparities in exclusionary discipline.

Other research has found similar patterns. One study gave K–12 teachers the behavior reports of two fictitious children and asked a series of questions about the teachers' perceptions of the children and their discipline recommendations. The behavior reports were identical, but researchers manipulated the race of the fictitious children by using stereotypical White (i.e., Greg and Jake) and Black (i.e., Darnell and DeShawn) names. Okonofua, Paunesku, and Walton (2016) found that after the second infraction, teachers were more likely to label Black relative to White children as “troublemakers,” which may imply attribution of the behaviors in the report to factors internal to the children and the belief that there was a higher likelihood that the behaviors would continue. Teachers were also more likely to recommend exclusionary discipline for Black children compared with their White peers after the second infraction (Okonofua & Eberhardt, 2015).

These education-specific studies build on a broader base of literature focused on implicit racial bias and its effects on behavior. Studies examining implicit racial bias have found that both White and Black subjects more readily describe White faces with positive words and Black faces with negative words (Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2005), and that Black men are rated as more angry and aggressive (Duncan, 1976; Hugenberg & Bodenhausen, 2003; Miller, Maner, & Becker, 2010). Research has even found that raising the subject of crime causes participants to think of Black men (Eberhardt et al., 2004). As discussed previously, children are often the subjects of these biases and perceptions.

Empathy has also been found to be an important dimension of teacher–child relationships and has been linked to the quality of those relationships as well as to discipline decisions. Research on empathy interventions demonstrates effects on reducing implicit racial bias of White teachers toward Black individuals (Whitford & Emerson, 2019), increasing empathetic mindsets about challenging behavior, and decreasing suspensions (Okonofua, Paunesku, & Walton, 2016).

Each of these biases and dynamics contributes to classroom climate overall and to teacher–child interactions and relationships. Overall, this body of work indicates that Black children, and in many cases Latino, Indigenous, immigrant, and dual language learner children, are subject to bias across an array of domains, including perceptions of behavior, expectations, and empathy. This bias contributes not only to the quality of teacher–child relationships and interactions but also more generally to reduced opportunities in learning settings that can shape children's learning experiences.

Pedagogy, Instruction, and Access to Enrichment

Schools serving predominantly low-income students of color tend to rely on narrow pedagogies and reductive instructional approaches that stress training in basic skills and reflect low expectations (Darling-Hammond & Baratz-Snowden, 2007). Certain school accountability frameworks, for instance, have negatively impacted educational opportunities for learners of color, as was evident most recently in the No Child Left Behind era. The emphasis on the use of test scores to track educational achievement across subgroups of students shaped the curricula and instruction students experienced. Researchers found, for example, that schools serving marginalized communities devoted an inordinate amount of time to teaching to the test and practicing test-taking skills; cheating practices were also documented (Nichols & Berliner, 2007). Students who were on the cusp of attaining proficiency scores received greater attention and resources relative to their lower-scoring peers, and teacher–student relationships were affected by the daily shuffling experienced by these students in rotating across remedial programs and interventions (Valli & Buese, 2007). Teacher morale declined as outcomes were reduced to test scores, and professional knowledge and judgment were devalued. Because school funding was linked to adequate yearly progress, schools and districts serving predominantly low-income populations of color were more susceptible to funding cuts, which in turn led to the imposition of a “failing school” label and increased the likelihood of teacher attrition (Darling-Hammond & Baratz-Snowden, 2007; Artiles, 2011). These developments substantially altered educational opportunities, including the quality of education and the nature of the curricula and pedagogy available to students of color. There is also evidence that test-based accountability led to lower-quality teachers being assigned to early grades, where students do not yet take standardized tests (Fuller & Ladd, 2013; Henry, McNeill, & Harbatkin, 2022).

As discussed in Chapter 2, a strong theoretical and empirical foundation supports learning through play, play-based pedagogy, and play-based instruction (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2009; Zosh et al., 2017, 2018; Parker & Stjerne Thomsen, 2019). Research indicates, however, that play is differentially allowed for children of color and those in low-income schools, and also is differentially perceived by adults depending on the demographics of the children engaging in play. Play has been described as a spectrum and defined according to various types, such as sociodramatic or free play, as well as its core features, including joy, active engagement, and meaningfulness (Burghardt, 2011; Zosh et al., 2017, 2018). Play-based learning can unfold through a variety of teacher- or child-led approaches (cooperative learning, project-based learning, inquiry-based learning; Parker & Stjerne Thomsen, 2019). Guided play, a concept that blends teacher- and child-directed learning, allows children to have autonomy and choice in play while ensuring that an adult provides opportunities for direct learning during play by, for example, asking open-ended or inquiry-based questions or exposing children to new concepts or vocabulary (Fisher et al., 2011; Skene et al., 2022). Research has found that this approach to learning engages children in activities that are meaningful to them, and thus intrinsically motivating. According to both research and several theoretical frameworks, learning that is meaningful to children is associated with increased attention, memory, and motivation (Piaget, 1972; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Dang et al., 2012; Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015; Zosh et al., 2017; Bodrova & Leong, 2019).

There is growing evidence that “community schools” produce positive student and school outcomes, particularly for marginalized learners (Maier et al., 2017). Community schools offer an “integrated focus on academics, health and social services, youth and community development, and community engagement” (Coalition for Community Schools, 2020, p. 1). The design of these schools is flexible and ambitious, offering extended-day and year-round schedules for children and adults. For obvious reasons, community schools offer invaluable resources and opportunities to students and families that face major structural barriers associated with racial and economic inequities. The design of these schools generally rests on four programmatic pillars: integrated student support, expanded learning time and opportunities, family and community engagement, and collaborative leadership and practice. Maier and colleagues (2017) reviewed more than 140 studies as well as various evaluations on the features of community schools. They identified areas that merit additional research, although their overall conclusion is that community schools “offer a promising foundation for progress” (Maier et al., 2017, p. 113).

High-quality and culturally responsive pedagogy and instruction are important factors in children's learning experiences. Access to enrichment programs, such as gifted and talented education, can also have positive effects on children's experiences and trajectories. Yet some research points to inequities in this area as well. For example, there is a long-standing debate about the underidentification of students of color in gifted and talented programs (Ford, 2021), characterized by deficit discourses focused on the ability levels and potential of these students and normative views of gifts and talent (Tenenbaum & Ruck, 2007; Ford, Grantham, & Whiting, 2008; Ford, 2021). Data indicate that Black and Latino students combined make up 41% of school enrollment but only 21% of students enrolled in gifted and talented programs (Patrick, Socol, & Morgan, 2020). A systematic review of referrals to these programs found that across all studies, teachers underreferred Black children (Ford, Grantham, and Whiting, 2008). Other research has found that even when researchers control for children's academic profiles, test scores, and socioeconomic backgrounds, Black children are still underreferred (Grissom & Redding, 2016), pointing to the role of bias in perceptions and expectations of children.

Research and theoretical development have also focused on culturally grounded pedagogy and learning. Several interrelated literatures use the notion of “culture”—in its dynamic and historical meanings—as the cornerstone of learning environments and interventions, encompassing culturally relevant, culturally responsive, and culturally sustaining approaches (Ladson-Billings, 1994; Gay, 2010; Paris, 2012).

Culturally relevant pedagogy, first introduced by Ladson-Billings (1995), centers on the experiences of children, including Black children and other children of color, who historically have been left out of education models. It focuses on changing mindsets and dispositions and ensuring high expectations and long-term academic success while promoting positive cultural and racial identity and enabling critical analysis of social inequalities within and outside of the classroom.

Culturally responsive teaching (Gay, 2010) builds on this theory but places greater emphasis on practice and competencies. It describes the ways teachers can realize culturally grounded learning by building on children's strengths and embracing families' cultural assets, knowledge, and prior experiences to make learning more relevant and engaging. The approach applies a social justice lens and focuses on achieving systemic and interpersonal change to promote success for learners of color. Most recently, scholars have introduced culturally sustaining pedagogy (Paris, 2012). This approach shifts the emphasis from drawing on children's culture for learning to sustaining and strengthening their connections to their culture and language through learning.

The above models for culturally grounded learning share a strengths-based perspective on children and families, with the goal of academic success, development of positive racial and cultural identity, and active engagement with inequality in the broader community. Research has found that many of these dimensions are associated with increased student engagement, persistence, attendance, and academic success (Hammond, 2015; Aronson & Laughter, 2016; Muñiz, 2020). Yet despite these positive outcomes, recent reviews of the literature on culturally relevant education show decreasing attention to this work (Aronson & Laughter, 2016), particularly for Native American students (Castagno & Brayboy, 2008).

THE IMPACT OF COVID-19

The closure of schools during the COVID-19 pandemic had a pronounced impact on families (Weiland et al., 2021). Parents reported high stress levels due to the shift to working from home and managing remote learning, and essential workers who needed to work in person struggled with providing care for their children. Research has shown that parents felt nervous, anxious, or apprehensive about the pandemic (Gonzalez et al., 2020). In addition, parents/guardians described the ways in which the pandemic caused major changes in their families' activities and routines (Gonzalez et al., 2020) and were more concerned about their children's social and emotional development and well-being than they had been prior to the pandemic (Jung & Barnett, 2021). The effects of interruptions in schooling were exacerbated by the disproportionate incidence of COVID-19 among some racial and minoritized groups: illness, hospitalization, and death due to COVID-19 have been higher among Hispanic or Latino, Black, American Indian or Alaska Native, and Native Hawaiian and other Pacific Islander populations (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020). As a result, children of color have been disproportionately affected by sickness and death among parents and other family members.

The shift to remote schooling during the pandemic introduced changes that were not conducive to social and academic learning (Weiland et al., 2021), and had a significant impact on children's learning opportunities and academic outcomes (EmpowerK12, 2020). Research has shown that many teachers were unable to teach/interact with children effectively online at the onset of the pandemic (Bassok et al., 2021). Reductions in instructional time and quality of instruction have also been documented (Rickles et al., 2020; Weiland et al., 2021). In line with those findings, parents nationally reported high levels of conduct problems, peer problems, and prosocial behavior problems among their children (Jung and Barnett, 2021), and children experienced slower learning growth or unrealized learning opportunities (Dorn et al., 2020; Huff, 2020; Renaissance Learning, 2020; Amplify, 2021; Engzell, Frey, & Verhagen, 2021; Lewis et al., 2021; McGinty et al., 2021; Ohio Department of Education, 2021; Storey & Zhang, 2021; Texas Education Agency, 2021; National Center for Education Statistics [NCES], 2022). A special administration of the National Assessment of Educational Progress in 2022 found disproportionately large declines in test scores in reading and math overall and inequitable outcomes among different groups of children as a result of the pandemic (NCES, 2022).

The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention surveyed parents of children aged 5–12 between October and November 2020 and found that, compared with parents of children attending school in person, parents of children receiving remote instruction were more likely to report higher levels of emotional distress, conflict between working and providing child care, and difficulty sleeping (Verlenden et al., 2021). Families adapted to the new circumstances with significant changes in their labor market participation (National Academies, 2023).

In addition to these effects, the COVID-19 pandemic has affected school enrollment, which may in turn lead to disparities in school funding. Progressive school funding allocations have been shown to have both short- and long-term effects on student outcomes, especially for students from families with low incomes. In fall 2020, many large school districts reported substantial declines in enrollment—especially in kindergarten, where enrollment decreased by an average of 16%. Schools also saw decreases in attendance during the switch to virtual learning in spring 2020. These declines can negatively affect school funding that is based on enrollment numbers, as well as funding for schools that use attendance measures to allocate funding, and are more likely to affect high-poverty compared with better-resourced districts (Blagg, Gutierrex, & Lee, 2021).

CONCLUSIONS

Although the early elementary system is universally accessible to children and families, gaps in access to quality remain pervasive. As with ECE, differences in quality and funding create disparities in children's experiences and outcomes. This unequal access to both structural and process quality can be influenced by such factors as geography, district size, school infrastructure, funding inequities, teacher and administrator bias, disciplinary policies, and other manifestations of marginalization based on disability status, race, gender, and socioeconomic status. These social and systemic drivers also affect the ways in which structural and process quality interact, leading to a broad spectrum of variation in access to resources and quality educational experiences for young children.

In some cases, federal funding for programs to address the needs of certain populations is insufficient to bridge gaps in state and local funding, resulting in underfunded programs. For students living in states that already have relatively low student spending, this funding gap results in more students at risk—especially students from families with lower incomes and students of color. In 2015, in states with higher levels of poverty, underfunding of Title I, which is meant to ensure that all children receive a quality and equitable education regardless of income, was just over one-eighth the amount required to fully fund the Basic Grants portion. As a result, students from states with higher levels of poverty have received less funding per low-income child relative to states with lower levels of poverty. In the case of IDEA, underfunding has left a large burden for state and local governments and families, affecting the quality, timing, and dosage of services children receive and the quality of coordination with other services.

Children from marginalized populations experience a variety of biases in the early grades related to their race/ethnicity, language status, and disability status that can manifest in differential teacher expectations, perceptions of behavior, and perception of age/maturity. These biases influence experiences in the classroom, relationships with teachers, exposure to harsh and exclusionary disciplinary practices, and other experiences in school that can lead to opportunity gaps and influence both short- and long-term outcomes. These biases can also lead to the misperception that some children do not need environments that help support their learning and development. For children with disabilities, biases can contribute to both under- and overidentification of disabilities, which can prevent children from receiving services to which they are entitled legally or result in children being diagnosed with conditions they do not have. In both cases, the result can be gaps in access to inclusive learning opportunities that adequately support student needs.

Well-funded, high-quality experiences in the early grades that follow well-funded and aligned ECE experiences can improve students' academic performance and in the longer term can lead to more positive outcomes, such as higher graduation rates and reduced adult poverty. It is important to ensure that these early elementary experiences are aligned with the latest science and are specifically designed to close opportunity gaps and ensure that all students succeed, particularly those who have been historically marginalized. These experiences include high-quality instruction and asset-driven pedagogies, assessments, and curricula; social-emotional and mental health supports and policies to explicitly reduce exclusionary and harsh discipline and eliminate disparities in such practices; full inclusion of children with disabilities in general education settings, with high-quality and individualized services and supports; bilingual learning opportunities for children who are English learners and dual language learners; structurally sound, safe, healthy, and engaging learning environments; a well-qualified, fairly compensated, and supported teacher workforce; data-driven, continuous quality improvement efforts targeted at identifying and addressing opportunity and outcome gaps; authentic and meaningful family engagement and partnerships; community partnerships and engagement to promote child and holistic family wellness; and strong partnerships with ECE systems that promote seamless transitions from ECE to the early grades.

REFERENCES

Footnotes

1

Racialization is defined as the act of giving a racial character to someone or something or the process of categorizing, marginalizing, or regarding according to race (Merriam-Webster, 2022).

2

Structural quality includes such factors as teacher:student ratios, class size, and teacher competencies and credentials.

3

Process quality includes such factors as teacher–child interactions and closeness of relationships between students and teachers.

Copyright 2023 by the National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Bookshelf ID: NBK596380

Views

  • PubReader
  • Print View
  • Cite this Page
  • PDF version of this title (5.5M)

Related information

  • PMC
    PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed
    Links to PubMed

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...