U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Bermúdez-Rattoni F, editor. Neural Plasticity and Memory: From Genes to Brain Imaging. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2007.

Cover of Neural Plasticity and Memory

Neural Plasticity and Memory: From Genes to Brain Imaging.

Show details

Chapter 14Neuro-Immune Associative Learning

, , , and .

14.1. INTRODUCTION

Experimental evidence demonstrates intensive and extensive interactions between the nervous and immune systems.1–4 The central capacity of associating a certain immune response or status (allergen, toxin, antigen) with a specific stimulus (environment or flavor) seems to be of high adaptive value; this special kind of associative learning may have been acquired as an adaptive strategy during evolution in order to protect an organism and/or prepare it for danger. Furthermore, it is possible that, depending on the different environmental challenges, the species formed species-specific associations during evolution.5

Classical conditioning or associative learning is often described as the transfer of the response-eliciting property of a biologically significant stimulus (unconditioned stimulus; US) to another stimulus (conditioned stimulus; CS) without that property.6–10 This transfer is thought to occur only if the CS serves as a predictor of the US.11–13 Thus, classical conditioning can be understood as learning about the temporal or causal relationships between external and internal stimuli, to allow for the appropriate preparatory set of responses before biologically significant events occur.

Regarding neuro-immune associative learning (NIAL), an immunomodulatory stimulus (antigen, immunomodulating drug) is employed as an US and paired with a neutral stimulus. After this associative phase, the neutral stimulus becomes a CS that can modify the immune response on demand (conditioned response; CR). The influence of NIAL on immune responses has been reviewed several times.14–32 However, in this chapter we propose an innovative approach, pointing out the biological meaning and possible clinical implications of neuro-immune associative learning. Furthermore, after analyzing the available literature, we propose a general theoretical framework for this special kind of associative learning.

14.1.1. Historical Development

S. Metalnikov and V. Chorine are generally credited with having conducted the first studies on NIAL.33 However, V.I. Luk’ianenko cites the 1911 dissertation of I.I. Makukhin at the University of St. Petersburg and a 1925 report by A. Voronov and I. Riskin as perhaps the first researchers to demonstrate the “conditioned leukocytic reaction.”17 Metalnikov and Chorine reported a series of experiments that clearly demonstrated the possibility of associating exteroceptive stimuli with alterations in immune parameters induced for the most part by injections of bacterial and viral preparations.33,34 Guinea pigs were given daily association trials with contingent pairing of scratching or heating of the skin as a CS and an immune challenge (i.p. injection of a small dose of tapioca, Bacillus anthrax, or a Staphylococcus filtrate) as an US. The CR was an increase in peritoneal leukocyte numbers that was weaker and more transitory than the UR. Follow-up experiments indicated that after the recall phase, conditioned animals survived longer after lethal injections of Vibrio cholera bacteria.

These initial results were rapidly replicated,35,37 and in the following years Soviet investigators paid considerable attention to this topic.17,38 Many of these experiments were basically similar to those performed by Metalnikov and Chorine, and apparently a controversy arose over the reproducibility of certain experiments. In parallel to the Soviet experiments, but not as well-known, were investigations performed in Romania39–41 and Switzerland42–47 reporting modulation of phagocytic activity and anaphylactic reaction, respectively, by evoking specific NIAL. In 1975, R. Ader and N. Cohen reported an immunosuppressive status after recalling the specific association of a taste stimulus (CS) and an immunosuppressive drug (US).48

Later, these researchers and other groups developed different NIAL protocols, most of them using taste and olfactory stimuli (CS) in rodents and humans. More than 30 years of clinical and experimental research have demonstrated that by evoking NIAL, the brain can suppress or enhance humoral as well as cellular immune responses (Table 14.1). These effects are biologically relevant for the organism, as they affect the course and outcome of disease and thus have possible applications in clinical settings.

TABLE 14.1

TABLE 14.1

Immune Parameters Affected after Recalling Neuro-Immune Associative Learning Paradigms

14.2. PHENOMENON: ASSOCIATION BETWEEN EXTEROCEPTIVE AND IMMUNE STIMULI

Two basic steps compose any conditioning protocol: an association phase in which one or more CS-US contingent pairings occur, inducing an associative learning process, and a recall phase in which the memory of such an association is retrieved after exposing the subject to the CS. Pioneer NIAL reports were based on the association of somatosensory stimulation (CS) and peripheral immune challenges (US). See Figure 14.1C. However, most of the recent work reporting conditioned effects on immune function has followed two basic associative protocols (Figure 14.1). The principal difference between them is the nature of the CS: taste/olfactory (Figure 14.1A) or visual/auditory (Figure 14.1B).

FIGURE 14.1. Neuro-immune associative learning.

FIGURE 14.1

Neuro-immune associative learning. A. After water regimen consumption is established, animals are exposed to a novel taste/olfactory conditioned stimulus (CS) paired with an immunomodulating unconditioned stimulus (US) generally administered intraperitoneally (more...)

Although conditioned stimuli may employ any of the exteroceptive sensory modalities (touch, vision, taste, olfaction, and audition), the naturalistic relation between the CS and the US may explain the feasibility and strength of a specific association.10

14.2.1. Taste- and Olfactory-Immune Associative Learning

Theoretically this protocol is based on the naturalistic relation of food and drink ingestion with possible immune consequences that may also induce behavioral modifications after the experience. On the experimental bench, the association step involves the pairing of a taste (e.g., saccharin), odor (e.g., camphor), or flavor (e.g., chocolate drink) as a CS with a stimulus that has immune consequences as a US (e.g., immunomodulating drug, or antigen), usually administered intraperitoneally. At recall time, subjects are normally exposed to the CS alone (Figure 14.1A) and some protocols employ a vehicle injection as an additional component of the CS.

Conditioned ingestive avoidance and aversion are often displayed after a single association trial; however, the conditioned effects on the immune system may not be evident until several association trials are applied.49 It is important to indicate that the behaviorally conditioned response (i.e., aversion or avoidance) has been elicited by NIAL in which T-dependent antigens such as protein antigens,50–52 T-independent antigens such as lipopolysaccharides,53–55 superantigens such as staphylococcal enterotoxin,56,57 immunosuppressive drugs such as cyclophosphamide,48,58 and cyclosporine A59,60 have been employed (Figure 14.2). Additionally, the magnitude of the behaviorally conditioned response seems to be modulated by the intensity of the immune stimulation at association time.

FIGURE 14.2. Neuro-immune associative learning differentially affects drinking behavior.

FIGURE 14.2

Neuro-immune associative learning differentially affects drinking behavior. Different immune stimuli were employed as unconditioned stimuli resulting in associative learning. klh = small dose of keyhole limpet hemocyanin. KLH = high dose of KLH (data (more...)

For instance, a dose–response relationship between the amount of antigen used (US) and the conditioned taste aversion has been demonstrated: the higher the antigen dose, the more pronounced the conditioned taste avoidance.51,61 Using a mild dose of antigen after an immune sensitization procedure induces a strong behaviorally conditioned avoidance response.62 Regarding immunosuppressive drugs, it has been documented that the immunosuppressive effects of cyclosporine A (US) can be associated with the taste of saccharin (CS). After recalling such an association, experimental subjects displayed immunosuppressive status (CR).30,60 However, different behaviorally conditioned responses result from such associative learning. In the case of mice, this specific taste-immune association does not result in conditioned taste avoidance behavior,5 whereas rats display reduced appetitive behavior,30 and for humans the palatability of the conditioned taste is affected.63

14.2.2. Visual- and Auditory-Immune Associative Learning

This protocol induces the association of a certain context and an immune response or status (Figure 14.1B). At association time, a certain context composed of visual and/or auditory cues (CS) is paired with a stimulus that has immune consequences (US). At recall time, the subject is exposed to the same context that induces the CR (Figure 14.1B). To induce conditioned effects on the immune system, it is usually necessary to submit the experimental subject to several association trials.

Aversive behavior to the context associated with the US may also be part of the CR displayed. For instance, an immune sensitization [s.c. injection of ovalbumin (OVA), i.p. injection of Bordetella pertussis, and larval infection with the nematode Nippostrongylus brasiliensis] induced higher levels of IgE after subsequent antigen injections and increased the number of intestinal mast cells.64 In subsequent trials, s.c. injections of OVA (US) were contingently paired with an auditory or visual cue (CS) in three association trials. One hour after a single recall trial (CS alone), rat mast cell protease (RMCP)-II levels in the serum were enhanced, suggesting that the degranulation of mucosal mast cells was behind the immune response.

Five hours after recall, serum RMCP-II levels did not significantly differ from control groups. A strong anaphylactic response in the lung as a conditioned response has recently been reported following a similar NIAL protocol.65 In addition, these authors reported high levels of stress and anxiety postrecall and demonstrated that the audio-visual stimulus (CS) was stressful and anxiogenic per se.

Another set of animal studies supports the associability of environmental stimuli (CS) with anaphylactic shock reaction.66–68 Rats sensitized with OVA were given injections of the same antigen to elicit a second anaphylactic shock in a context different from that in which the first anaphylactic shock was induced. Animals experiencing both anaphylactic shocks in the same context displayed much smaller shocks than rats subjected to the second shock in a different context. Repeated, non-reinforced presentation of the antigen in the new context prior to the CS and induction of the first anaphylactic shock prevented the increased resistance to the induction of a second anaphylactic shock reaction. These findings were ascribed to the CR that attenuated the UR (anaphylactic shock).

Accordingly, it has recently been shown that OVA-immunized mice avoid the context previously associated with presentation of the allergen against which they have been immunized.69 In a modified classical passive avoidance test, OVA aerosol was employed as an aversive stimulus (US); although attracted by the supposedly safer, dark compartment of the apparatus (CS), OVA-immunized mice avoided entering the dark side, preferring the bright (usually aversive) side of the box.

When CNS activity was tracked using c-Fos expression as a neuronal metabolic marker, allergic animals showed enhanced c-Fos immunoreactivity in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus and central nucleus of the amygdala after airway OVA challenge. These brain structures are commonly linked to emotional and affective behavioral patterns that are important components in the development of learned aversive behavior such as conditioned taste aversion.70

14.2.3. Touch-Immune Associative Learning

With touch-immune associative learning, somatosensory stimulation (scratching, heating the skin) can be employed as a CS paired with a stimulus that has immune consequences (US). At recall time, the subject is exposed to the CS that induces the CR and affects peripheral immune functions (Figure 14.1C). To our knowledge, apart from pioneer studies in the 1920s33,34 and their replications35–37 (see Section 14.1.1), no further attempts have been made to develop NIAL employing somatosensory stimulation as a CS. Furthermore, it should be noted that behavioral control conditions and immunological interpretations have changed substantially since those experiments were reported.

14.3. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK FOR NEURO-IMMUNE ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING

After reviewing the existing data about NIAL, it is possible to summarize guidelines for the general mechanisms underlying these phenomena (summarized in Figure 14.3). Part of this conceptualization has already been elaborated.28,71,72 According to this theory, in the terminology of behavioral conditioning, both the CS (changes in the external environment) and the US (changes in the internal environment) must be inputs to the CNS, which in turn processes and associates this information. Thus, at association time, only a change in the immune system sensed by the CNS can serve as a US. Furthermore, both the CR and the UR must be outputs of the CNS. Thus, only an immune parameter directed by the CNS can serve as a UR for conditioning and at recall time the CR will resemble such a UR.

FIGURE 14.3. Theoretical framework for neuro-immune associative learning.

FIGURE 14.3

Theoretical framework for neuro-immune associative learning. At association time two possible unconditioned stimuli (US) were associated with a conditioned stimulus (CS). The US directly detected by the CNS is defined as a directly perceived US. The one (more...)

14.3.1. Association Phase

Two possible kinds of US are employed to induce NIAL. The US that is directly detected by the CNS is defined as a directly perceived US, whereas the one that needs one or more intermediary molecules released by another system in order to be detected by the CNS is an indirectly perceived US. Any US perceived directly or indirectly has two possible afferent pathways to the CNS: (1) a neural afferent pathway and (2) a humoral afferent pathway. The neural afferent pathway may detect the US and translate this information into neural activity. This sensory process implies the interoceptive capacities, including immunoception, of the CNS.73,74 Theoretically, this afferent pathway may also be able to codify the location of stimulation (local versus systemic).

The humoral afferent pathway is required for any US that is not detected locally or for molecules induced by a given US (indirectly perceived US) that reach the CNS via the blood stream. If a given US has effects on several cell types, it is reasonable to assume that several molecules are candidates to be perceived by the CNS (directly perceived US). Figure 14.3 indicates that for an indirectly perceived US, the pathway to the CNS is complex and longer than for a directly perceived US. After administration of a given US, it can be hypothesized that the CNS takes longer to respond to indirectly perceived unconditioned stimuli than to those perceived directly. This feature can be employed in experimental designs in order to elucidate the nature of the US. For instance, backward conditioning should not be possible in the case of a directly perceived US, whereas an indirectly perceived US could result in associative learning under such conditions.

For an indirectly perceived US that affects immune functions, one should consider that the immune history (tolerance and memory) of a subject may interfere with the response to such a US, resulting in a different immune reaction, and thus a different signal intensity to the CNS. For example, the immune responses to a given antigen differ completely between the first and second times because an immune memory process takes place after the first exposition. Another example is the immune tolerance phenomenon developed after repeated exposition to the same drug. Applying the same conditioning protocol to two subjects with varying immune histories may result in different associative learning processes that in turn may affect the immune system in diverse ways at recall time.

In Pavlovian conditioning, the strength of the association of the CS and the US is affected by the temporal relation (inter-stimulus interval) between these stimuli. Orthodox theory predicts that when the US precedes the presentation of the CS (backward conditioning), learning is poor. In addition, when the CS precedes the US by increasing intervals, the probability of a CR declines.13 The specific inter-stimulus interval that yields the most pronounced CR varies with organisms and responses studied. For NIAL, this associative feature may be employed to delineate the nature of the US (directly versus indirectly perceived) by systematically varying the inter-stimulus interval between the CS and US and describing the UR kinetics.

Regarding the CS, it is important to mention that features such as novelty, intensity, duration, and naturalistic relation to the US may explain the feasibility of associating the CS with the US, as well as the stability and strength of such an association. For instance, it is well documented that gustatory or olfactory stimuli are strongly and easily associated with visceral US, in contrast to tactile, visual, or auditory stimuli.10,75 The strength and lastingness of flavor-visceral association is reflected in several features of the CR, such as high magnitude and low forgetting rate or extinction resistance. Additionally, the neutrality of the CS toward the immune system must be assured for each experimental setting.

14.3.2. Recall Phase

The CR represents tacit and unique proof that an associative learning process has occurred at association time. In several NIAL protocols, the experimental subjects displayed complex CRs with behavioral and neuroendocrine components in addition to the effects observed in the immune system. Furthermore, it is necessary to consider that the immune system displays sensitization (memory) and habituation (tolerance) responses to specific stimuli and that many immune parameters underlie circadian rhythms. Thus, the specific time points for association and recall and the immunological history of each experimental subject may be important variables for the final immunological outcome after recalling a given NIAL. Thus, some immune parameters modulated at recall time may be the bizarre reflections of neural activities not explained by established learning and memory rules.

For example, the expression of neural activity (e.g., behavior) may follow a normal extinction process during consecutive recall trials, whereas the effects of such activity on the peripheral immune response may not display the same extinction slope or may even be enhanced. Such peculiar results may be explained in several ways. It is possible that the interval between the recall trials is not long enough for the specific immune measure to return to baseline, resulting in an additive effect on the conditioned immune response during consecutive near-recall trials. Another possibility is related to immune processes that may be modulated by neural activity; however, once such a process has started it is basically independent of neural modulation. Additionally, it has been demonstrated that a stable and sturdy memory under extinction can to a great extent be reconsolidated by contingent exposure to the US, even if it is of low intensity.76

Thus it is possible that, after an intensive association phase, the conditioned immune effects work as a putative US, inducing a reconsolidation process of the memory trace at each recall trial and therefore maintaining or enhancing the conditioned effects on the immune system in subsequent recall trials. In this regard, it should be indicated that some changes in the immune response after recalling NIAL cannot strictly be called CRs.71 In summary, immune responses can be affected by NIAL, but this does not necessarily imply that such immune responses were behaviorally conditioned.

Regarding the conditioned immune response, specific features may give important clues to its nature and should be considered in an experimental design in order to differentiate the underlying mechanisms. The two possible pathways by which the CNS can modulate immune functions are (1) the humoral efferent pathway and (2) the neural efferent pathway. The humoral efferent pathway may imply changes in neuro-hormones that directly or indirectly modify the immune response at recall time. The peripheral effects evoked after activation of this pathway are diffuse and long-lasting like any neuroendocrine responses. The neural efferent pathway is supported by the direct innervations of primary and secondary lymphoid organs.77,78

Several immune parameters are subject to neural modulation: T cell differentiation,79,80 hematopoiesis,81–83 T cell activity, B cell activity,84 natural killer (NK) cell activity,85–87 and inflammatory responses.3,88,89 We hypothesize that many of these neuro-immune interactions may be affected by NIAL but few have been investigated experimentally to date.

The extinction of the conditioned immune response is another feature that may give hints about the underlying mechanisms. It has been demonstrated that for behavioral conditioning in which the nervous system directly modulates the CR (e.g., nictitating membrane, gastric secretion, aversive behavior), an extinction process occurs when the CS is repeatedly presented without the US (active forgetting). However, if the CR observed is the reflection of neural activity on different types of cells, like immune cells that exhibit tolerance and memory processes, CR extinction will not necessarily be elicited in an orthodox Pavlovian manner. Moreover, if the CR observed reflects neural activity on two or more cellular types, the picture may be more abnormal (e.g., the neuroendocrine efferent pathway affecting immune functions). Further consideration is required since the life spans of immune cells are short and thus each recall trial may affect different leukocyte or lymphocyte subsets.

Several experimental approaches allow analysis of the mechanisms responsible for behavioral conditioning. For example, treatment performed before association may affect sensory and early associative processes (short-term memory). Consolidation and long-term memory can be experimentally analyzed by varying the timing of treatment after association. If the treatment is given before recall, then the memory process can be studied. This scheme seems to be applicable for short-term treatments. However, for long-lasting treatment, differentiating between the learning and the memory processes is only possible by comparing the effects of treatment given before association with those of treatment given before recall. In addition, the specific timing of a given treatment may depend on the nature of the treatment (short- versus long-term effects) and the pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of the drugs applied.

14.4. NEUROBIOLOGY OF NEURO-IMMUNE ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING

The naturalistic associability of food and drink ingestion with its possible immune consequences has been experimentally appraised in rodents and humans employing the conditioned taste aversion paradigm.90 Conditioned taste aversion or avoidance is a type of associative conditioning in which the subjects learn to associate a taste with delayed malaise.70 This learning has been conserved across the animal kingdom91–94 including humans95 which reflects its high adaptive value in food selection strategies. However, reduced ingestive behavior may be only part of a complex and diverse repertory of physiological responses that the individual evokes to avoid, reject and/or prepare to counteract the unconditioned effects.5

One discrete neural network involved in taste-visceral associative learning has already been described, mainly including sensory and hedonic neural pathways.96,97 Such a neural circuit consistently includes the nucleus tractus solitary, the parabrachial nucleus, medial thalamus, amygdala, and insular cortex.98 In particular, the insular cortex is essential for the acquisition and retention of this associative learning99,100 and it has been postulated that the insular cortex may integrate gustatory and visceral stimuli.101 More recently, using the neuronal activity marker c-Fos, it was possible to confirm the preponderant role of the insular cortex in conditioned antibody production102 in agreement with a previous report.103

Regarding other forebrain structures, the amygdala seems to play an important role during the formation of aversive ingestive associations104 and is also relevant for limbic–autonomic interactions.105 A series of reports indicates that the insular cortex and amygdala are key structures in conditioned immunosuppression after evoking taste-cyclophosphamide association.106,107

It has also been proposed that the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus, widely recognized as a satiety center,108 is intimately associated with sympathetic facilitation in peripheral tissues109 including modulation of peripheral immune reactivity.110 In agreement with previous reports,103,106,107 we have identified the neural substrates involved in behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression (CS: saccharin; US: cyclosporine A) in rats.111 The conditioned effect on the immune system reducing splenocyte responsiveness and cytokine production (IL-2 and IFN-α) was affected by brain excitotoxic lesions; this shows that the insular cortex is essential to acquiring and evoking this conditioned response. In contrast, the amygdala seems to mediate the input of visceral information necessary at acquisition time, whereas the ventro-medial hypothalamic nucleus appears to participate in the output pathway to the immune system needed to evoke a behaviorally conditioned immune response.

Using a pharmacological approach, the neurochemical features of the conditioned effect enhancing NK cell activity in rodents have been described. Central catecholamines seem to be essential, and glutamate — but not GABA — is also required at the recall stage.112,113 Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that cholinergic and serotonergic central systems are required at the association and recall stages.114

In addition to classical neurotransmitters, cytokines have been demonstrated to play an important role within the CNS, modulating neuronal and glial functions in non-pathological settings such as learning and memory processes.2,115,116 Specifically, pro-inflammatory cytokines such as IL-1, IL-6, and TNF-α have been shown to modulate spatial learning tasks and long-term potentiation phenomena.117–124 In this sense, it is plausible that cytokines are significant factors in the associative processes occurring during behavioral conditioning of immune functions.

Apart from these neuromodulatory properties, pro-inflammatory cytokines seem to play an important part in the afferent pathway between the immune system and the CNS.2,125,126 Therefore, it can be hypothesized that central cytokines act as mediators in the brain during “immune-sensing” in the association phases of behaviorally conditioned immunomodulating paradigms. These hypotheses are supported by observations that (1) receptors for these pro-inflammatory cytokines are expressed in the CNS,127,128 (2) peripheral immune changes affect central cytokine production and cytokine receptor expression in the brain,129,130 and (3) cytokines can act as unconditioned stimuli to induce conditioned taste aversion or avoidance.131–134 Apart from these reports, we know of no systematic attempts to elucidate the neural substrates underlying immunomodulating effects based on neuro-immune associative learning.

14.5. BIOLOGICAL RELEVANCE OF NEURO-IMMUNE ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING

Neuro-immune interactions appear to bring several adaptive advantages to those organisms that acquired and further developed them during ontogeny and phylogeny.94,135,136 This complex repertoire of physiological responses including immune, endocrine, neural, and behavioral responses may be orchestrated to achieve better adaptation of the organism to a constantly challenging environment. In vertebrates, the many intricate interactions in both directions between the immune and nervous systems are well established.137–140 It was recently shown that invertebrate biology also evolved around acquiring and developing complex neuro-immune communications.

For example, interaction between neurons and immune cells has been demonstrated in the mollusk Aplysia californica.141 Furthermore, invertebrates also express neuropeptides (e.g., opioids) in the neural and immune tissues that play a key role as neuro-immune messengers during their evolution.137 Neuro-immune complexity appears as well in the behavior of insects; for example, in the linkage between the immune and nervous systems of bees and humblebees.142,143 Noninfected honey bees whose immune systems were challenged by a nonpathogenic immunogenic elicitor (lipopolysaccharide) displayed reduced abilities to associate an odor with sugar reward in a classical conditioning paradigm.

Classical conditioning can be understood as learning about the temporal or causal relationships between external and internal stimuli to allow for the appropriate preparatory set of responses before biologically significant events.13,144 The capacity to associate a certain immune response or status (allergen, toxin, antigen) with a specific stimulus (environment or flavor) is of high adaptive value.

Thus, it can be hypothesized that this capacity was acquired during evolution as an adaptive strategy in order to protect organisms and/or prepare them to face danger. Furthermore, such associative learning is typically acquired under certain stressful conditions. For example, the exposure to a specific antigen (and its categorization as an allergen) may be associated (learning) with a specific environment or food. An adaptive response is then elicited (memory), consisting first of behavioral modification in order to avoid the place or food associated with the antigen.66,69

If this is not possible, the organism will try to reduce contact with the allergen, i.e., by coughing or sneezing.145 At the same time, its immune system may prepare the body for interaction with the antigen by mast cell degranulation64,65,146,147 or antibody production.50,102,148,149 Although under experimental conditions such associative learning can be extinguished, it is likely that it will last for a long time since an organism in a natural situation will try to avoid contact with the environmental cues that signal the CS.

14.6. CLINICAL RELEVANCE OF NEURO-IMMUNE ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING

Few attempts have been undertaken to specifically investigate conditioned effects that directly modulate peripheral immune functions in human subjects. Since the 19th century, anecdotic case studies have reported the occurrence of allergic symptoms in the absence of allergens provoked simply by different cues (i.e., conditioned stimuli) such as a picture of a hay field or an artificial rose.150 Several decades later, researchers reported conditioned dermatitis responses in adolescent male subjects (n = 4) resulting from evocation of a specific association (CS: blue solution application; US: 2% raw extract R. Venicifera application).151

In another case report, two asthmatic patients suffering from skin sensitivities to house-dust extract and grass pollen were exposed to these allergens by inhalation.152 After a series of conditioning trials, they experienced allergic attacks after inhalation of the neutral solvent used to deliver the allergens. This work showed not only fast conditioning of the asthmatic attack (CR), but also tenacious retention, i.e., lack of extinction. This observation along with data from animal experiments resulted in the early hypothesis that asthma could be conceived of as a learned response.153 This view was further supported by a conditioning protocol (CS: taste; US: dust mite allergen) in nine patients with allergic rhinitis.154 After the association phase, elevated mast cell tryptase in mucosa was observed when an intranasal saline application was given simultaneously with the CS.

Another type of allergic reaction, the delayed-type hypersensitivity response, was tested in seven healthy volunteers who received five monthly tuberculin skin tests.155 In this conditioning protocol both tuberculin (US) and saline were injected; while the latter was taken from a green vial (CS–), tuberculin was drawn from a red vial (CS+). On the test day, the color labeling of the substances was reversed. Although the saline injections did not induce skin reactions (erythema and induration), the severity of the symptoms was significantly blunted in all the subjects tested when the tuberculin was drawn from the green vial (conditioned effect). However, a similar protocol using various allergens (mite dust, fur) taken from colored vials did not result in conditioned modulation of skin reactions in the 15 subjects tested.156

Associative learning has been consistently reported in the context of cancer treatment, particularly chemotherapy.28 Chemotherapy agents (e.g., cyclophosphamide) generally have immunosuppressive effects. They are typically administered in cycles, with each outpatient treatment infusion followed by a period of recovery prior to the next infusion. From a conditioning perspective, clinic treatment visits can be viewed as association trials in which the distinctive salient features of the clinic environment (CS) are contingently paired with the infusion of agents (US: cyclophosphamide) that have effects on the immune system.

Immune function was assessed in 20 cancer patients in a hospital prior to chemotherapy and compared with assessments conducted at home. Proliferative responses to T cell mitogens were lower for cells isolated from blood samples taken in the hospital (after recall) than for home samples.157 These results were replicated in 22 ovarian patients158 and 19 pediatric patients receiving chemotherapy.159 However, chemotherapy patients often develop conditioned nausea,157,160–162 anxiety,163,164 and fatigue165 responses to reminders of chemotherapy. These conditioned nausea and anxiety responses can also be elicited by thoughts and images of chemotherapy,166,167 raising the possibility that conditioned effects may affect patients during the course of normal life for years after treatment.

Only a few human studies have tried to affect immune parameters on the cellular level by employing behavioral conditioning procedures. Based on the knowledge that adrenaline administration leads to the immediate mobilization of leukocytes in the periphery, especially of NK cell numbers with simultaneous augmentation of their lytic activity,168,169 one research group assessed the conditionability of NK cell numbers and their lytic activity in healthy volunteers. Although positive results were reported after evoking a taste (CS)–adrenaline (US) association.170,171 these effects could not be replicated by other research groups.172

The efficacy of a conditioning protocol was also tested in multiple sclerosis patients, for whom four monthly cyclophosphamide infusions (US) were contingently paired with the taste of anise-flavored syrup (CS).173 Long-term treatment with cyclophosphamide decreased blood leukocyte numbers, often leading to leukopenia. Interestingly, after 6 months of administering a placebo infusion paired with the drink, 8 of 10 patients showed conditioned reductions in peripheral leukocyte numbers. In addition, by pairing s.c. interferon-α injections (US) with a distinctively flavored drink (CS), it was possible to induce elevations of neopterin and quinolinic acid serum levels after evoking such an association in healthy volunteers (n = 10).174

It has been hypothesized that more than a single associative learning trial pairing a distinctive taste (CS) with interferon-β injections (US) is necessary to produce immune conditioned effects.175 This view is supported by experimental data for healthy male volunteers (n = 18). The immunosuppressive drug cyclosporine A (US) was paired four times with a distinctively flavored and colored solution (CS),63 inducing taste-immune associative learning. The immunopharmacological mechanism of cyclosporine A involves its binding to cyclophilins, which leads to intracellular phosphatase calcineurin inhibition, then selectively reducing the expression of interleukin-2 (IL-2) and interferon- (IFN-α) cytokines, which finally resulted in specific suppression of T cell function.176 After association, the mere re-exposure to the drink (CS) induced conditioned inhibition of ex vivo cytokine (IL-2 and IFN-α) mRNA expression and cytokine release along with the proliferative responsiveness of human peripheral blood lymphocytes similar to the drug effect.

14.7. SUMMARY AND FUTURE PERSPECTIVE

Conceptualizing Pavlovian conditioning as a mechanism by which an organism anticipates the onset of a biologically important event (US) and initiates preparatory responses (CR) to allow the organism to deal better with US effects invites the hypothesis that one reason for the neural control of immunity lies in accommodating the adaptive value of classical conditioning.18 In its natural environment, an animal with a cut or a scratch must build up immunological defenses against microorganisms. In the laboratory or a clinical setting, an antigen is reliably preceded by an injection. Therefore, conditioned immune effects may, in fact, be very common.

The difficulty for the investigator lies not so much in inducing such responses, but in employing the proper controls, both immunological and psychological, in order to demonstrate that these responses exist and to explore the underlying mechanisms. Due to the physiological basis of the conditioned effects, the magnitude of the conditioned immune response should not to be expected to override the homeostatic balance of the organism. However, this does not mean that conditioned effects on immune functions are not of biological and clinical significance, as reviewed here and in previous work.27

A very small increase in the potential of the immune system may be of great value in the fight against pathogens when a system reaches an allostatic load,177,178 but it may increase the occurrence and severity of allergies and autoimmune disorders in other conditions. It is important to emphasize that several immune responses may be affected by NIAL protocols, but this does not necessarily imply that such immune responses are conditioned. Because of the complexity of neuro-immune interaction, such differentiation is not easy to establish.

As we have seen, the use of a US with immune consequences, such as immunomodulating drugs or antigens, is not the only requirement for genuinely conditioning an immune response through behavioral protocol. Experimental data reflect a dichotomy that is possibly supported by different mechanisms that may follow different rules. In our own experience and after reviewing the available literature, we conclude that the direction of the CR is a key feature of conditioning; i.e., the direction of this response should be independent of the immune, endocrine, and circadian status of the subject at association and recall time.

Before NIAL can be implemented as supportive therapy together with traditional pharmacological regimens, it is essential to describe some of its features. For example, we do not yet know how long conditioned immune responses last and how immune-specific they are. Since it may be necessary to apply reinforcement at appropriate intervals, questions arise whether reconditioning is possible. Therapy will eventually stop. What is the forgetting pattern of conditioned immune responses? How predictable is the conditioned immune response in a human population with different immune and psychological histories? What are the impacts of age and gender on immunoconditioning? When using immunomodulating drugs as the unconditioned stimuli, are some side effects conditioned?

To date, experimental evidence indicates that behavioral conditioning may have practical implications in a clinical setting and be of use as supportive therapy, with the aim of reducing undesired side effects and maximizing the effects of pharmacological therapies.179 In summary, we have reviewed and summarized the current data indicating that both innate and adaptive immune responses are affected by evoking neuro-immune associative learning. The effect of NIAL on immune functions is just about to be understood and the possible clinical applications seem to be enormous.

In future studies it will be essential to analyze the afferent and efferent pathways in brain-to-immune communications before NIAL paradigms can be employed as beneficial tools in clinical settings. Finally, research on behavioral immunoconditioning has revealed that organisms have important adaptive psychoneuroimmunological strategies acquired to deal with constantly changing and challenging environments in a better way.

REFERENCES

1.
Elmquist JK, Scammell TE, Saper CB. Mechanisms of CNS response to systemic immune challenge: the febrile response. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:565. [PubMed: 9416669]
2.
Dantzer R. Cytokine-induced sickness behaviour: a neuroimmune response to activation of innate immunity. Eur J Pharmacol. 2004;500:399. [PubMed: 15464048]
3.
Tracey K. The inflammatory reflex. Nature. 2002;420:853. [PubMed: 12490958]
4.
Glaser R, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Stress-induced immune dysfunction: implications for health. Nat Rev Immunol. 2005;5:243. [PubMed: 15738954]
5.
Niemi MB, et al. Murine taste-immune associative learning. Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20:527. [PubMed: 16631347]
6.
Hawkins R, et al. A cellular mechanism of classical conditioning in Aplysia: activity-dependent amplification of presynaptic facilitation. Science. 1983;219:400. [PubMed: 6294833]
7.
Pavlov I. Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. Oxford University Press; Cambridge: 1927. [PMC free article: PMC4116985] [PubMed: 25205891]
8.
Fanselow M, Poulos A. The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;56:207. [PubMed: 15709934]
9.
Carew T, Sahley C. Invertebrate learning and memory: from behavior to molecules. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9:435. [PubMed: 2423010]
10.
Domjan M. Pavlovian conditioning: a functional perspective. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;56:179. [PubMed: 15709933]
11.
Rescorla R, Wagner A. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: variations in the efectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black A, Prokasy W, editors. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. Appleton-Century-Crofts; New York: 1972. p. 64.
12.
Pearce J. A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. Psychol Rev. 1987;94:61. [PubMed: 3823305]
13.
Rescorla R. Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1988;11:329. [PubMed: 3284445]
14.
Ader R, Cohen N. Conditioned immunopharmacologic responses. In: Ader R, editor. Psychoneuroimmunology. 1. Academic Press; New York: 1981. p. 185.
15.
Ader R, Cohen N. The influence of conditioning on immune responses. In: Ader R, Felten D, Cohen N, editors. Pschoneuroimmunology. 2. Academic Press; New York: 1991. p. 611.
16.
Ader R, Cohen N. Conditioning and immunity. In: Ader R, Felten D, Cohen N, editors. Psychoneuroimmunology. 3. Academic Press; New York: 2001. p. 3.
17.
Luk’ianenko V. The problem of conditioned reflex regulation of immunobiologic reactions. USP Sovrem Biol. 1961;51:170. [PubMed: 13764186]
18.
Brittain R, Wiener N. Neural and Pavlovian influences on immunity. Pavlov J Biol Sci. 1985;20:181. [PubMed: 2866484]
19.
Spector N. Old and new strategies in the conditioning of immune responses. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1987;496:522. [PubMed: 2440373]
20.
Kusnecov A, King M, Husband A. Immunomodulation by behavioural conditioning. Biol Psychol. 1989;28:25. [PubMed: 2675991]
21.
Markovic B, Dimitrijevic M, Jankovic B. Immunomodulation by conditioning: recent developments. Int J Neurosci. 1993;71:231. [PubMed: 8407149]
22.
Hiramoto R, et al. The use of conditioning to probe for CNS pathways that regulate fever and NK cell activity. Int J Neurosci. 1996;84:229. [PubMed: 8707485]
23.
Spector N. Neuroimmunomodulation: a brief review: can conditioning of natural killer cell activity reverse cancer and/or aging? Regul Toxicol Pharmacol. 1996;24:S32. [PubMed: 8921553]
24.
Hiramoto R, et al. Psychoneuroendocrine immunology: site of recognition, learning and memory in the immune system and the brain. Int J Neurosci. 1997;92:259. [PubMed: 9522270]
25.
Hiramoto R, et al. Psychoneuroendocrine immunology: perception of stress can alter body temperature and natural killer cell activity. Int J Neurosci. 1999;98:95. [PubMed: 10395364]
26.
Spector N. The NIM revolution. Rom J Physiol. 1999;36:127. [PubMed: 11797929]
27.
Ader R. Conditioned immunomodulation: research needs and directions. Brain Behav Immun. 2003;17(Suppl 1):S51. [PubMed: 12615187]
28.
Bovbjerg D. Conditioning, cancer, and immune regulation. Brain Behav Immun. 2003;17 (Suppl 1):S58. [PubMed: 12615188]
29.
Hucklebridge F. Behavioral conditioning of the immune system. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2002;52:325. [PubMed: 12498110]
30.
Exton M, et al. Conditioning in the rat: an in vivo model to investigate the molecular mechanisms and clinical implications of brain-immune communication. Immunol Rev. 2001;184:226. [PubMed: 12086315]
31.
Exton M, et al. Pavlovian conditioning of immune function: animal investigation and the challenge of human application. Behav Brain Res. 2000;110:129. [PubMed: 10802310]
32.
Pacheco-López G, et al. Expectations and associations that heal: immunomodulatory placebo effects and neurobiology. Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20:430. [PubMed: 16887325]
33.
Metalnikov S, Chorine V. Role des réflexes conditionnels dans l’immunité Ann Inst Pasteur. 1926;11:893.
34.
Metalnikov S, Chorine V. Role des réflexes conditionnels dans la formation des anticorps. Compt R Seances Soc Biol Ses Fil. 1928;99:142.
35.
Nicolau I, Antinescu-Dimitriu O. Rôle des réflexes conditionnels dans la formation des anticorps. Compt R Seances Soc Biol Ses Fil. 1929;102:133.
36.
Nicolau I, Antinescu-Dimitriu O. L’influence des réflexes conditionnels sur l’exsudat peritonéal. Compt R Seances Soc Biol Ses Fil. 1929;102:144.
37.
Ostravskaya O. Le réflex conditionnel et les réactions de l’immunité Ann Inst Pasteur. 1930;44:340.
38.
Ader R. A historical account of conditioned immunobiologic responses. In: Ader R, editor. Psychoneuroimmunology. Academic Press; New York: 1981. p. 321.
39.
Baciu I, et al. Sur les centres nerveux régulateurs de l’erythropoièse. Rev Roum Physiol. 1965;2:123.
40.
Benetato G. Central nervous mechanism of the leukocytic and phagocytic reaction. J Physiol (Paris). 1955;47:391. [PubMed: 13264103]
41.
Benetato G, et al. Despre rolul scoartie cerebrale in activarea sistemului fagocitar si mobilizarea anticorpilor. Studi Cercetari Fil Acad RPR Chij. 1952;8:264.
42.
Noelpp B, Noelpp-Eschenhagen I. Experimental bronchial asthma in guinea pigs. Part II. The role of conditioned reflexes in the pathogenesis of bronchial asthma. Int Arch Allergy Appl Immunol. 1951;2:321. [PubMed: 14927234]
43.
Noelpp B, Noelpp-Eschenhagen I. Experimental bronchial asthma in guinea pigs. Part I. Methods for objective registration of asthma attacks. Int Arch Allergy Appl Immunol. 1951;2:308. [PubMed: 14927233]
44.
Noelpp B, Noelpp-Eschenhagen I. Role of conditioned reflex in bronchial asthma: experimental investigation on the pathogenesis of bronchial asthma. Helv Med Acta. 1951;18:142. [PubMed: 14831859]
45.
Noelpp B, Noelpp-Eschenhagen I. Experimental bronchial asthma in guinea pigs. Part III. Studies on the significance of conditioned reflexes: ability to develop conditioned reflexes and their duration under stress. Int Arch Allergy Appl Immunol. 1952;3:108. [PubMed: 13022137]
46.
Noelpp B, Noelpp-Eschenhagen I. Experimental bronchial asthma in guinea pigs. Part IV. Experimental asthma in the guinea pig as an experimental model. Int Arch Allergy Appl Immunol. 1952;3:207. [PubMed: 13044315]
47.
Noelpp B, Noelpp-Eschenhagen I. Experimental bronchial asthma in guinea pigs. Part V. Experimental pathophysiological studies. Int Arch Allergy Appl Immunol. 1952;3:302. [PubMed: 13044325]
48.
Ader R, Cohen N. Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression. Psychosom Med. 1975;37:333. [PubMed: 1162023]
49.
Espinosa E, et al. Enhancement of antibody response by one-trial conditioning: contrasting results using different antigens. Brain Behav Immun. 2004;18:76. [PubMed: 14651949]
50.
Husband AJ, et al. A conditioning model for immunostimulation: enhancement of the antibody responses to ovalbumin by behavioral conditioning in rats. In: Husband AJ, editor. Psychoimmunology. CRC Press; Boca Raton: 1993. p. 139.
51.
Pacheco-López G, et al. Peripheral protein immunization induces rapid activation of the CNS, as measured by c-Fos expression. J Neuroimmunol. 2002;131:50. [PubMed: 12458036]
52.
Djuric V, et al. Conditioned taste aversion in rats subjected to anaphylactic shock. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1987;496:561. [PubMed: 3474989]
53.
Bull DF, et al. Modulation of body temperature through taste aversion conditioning. Physiol Behav. 1991;49:1229. [PubMed: 1654573]
54.
Janz LJ, et al. Pavlovian conditioning of LPS-induced responses: effects on corticosterone, splenic NE, and IL-2 production. Physiol Behav. 1996;59:1103. [PubMed: 8737899]
55.
Cross-Mellor SK, Kavaliers M, Ossenkopp KP. Comparing immune activation (lipopolysaccharide) and toxin (lithium chloride)-induced gustatory conditioning: lipopolysaccharide produces conditioned taste avoidance but not aversion. Behav Brain Res. 2004;148:11. [PubMed: 14684243]
56.
Pacheco-López G, et al. Behavioral endocrine immune-conditioned response is induced by taste and superantigen pairing. Neuroscience. 2004;129:555. [PubMed: 15541877]
57.
Kusnecov A, Goldfarb Y. Neural and behavioral responses to systemic immunologic stimuli: a consideration of bacterial T cell superantigens. Curr Pharm Des. 2005;11:1039. [PubMed: 15777253]
58.
Lambert JV, Whitehouse WG. Conditioned inhibition of cyclophosphamide-induced taste aversion. J Gen Psychol. 2002;129:68. [PubMed: 12038496]
59.
Exton MS, et al. Conditioned taste aversion produced by cyclosporine A: concomitant reduction in lymphoid organ weight and splenocyte proliferation. Physiol Behav. 1998;63:241. [PubMed: 9423965]
60.
Klosterhalfen S, Klosterhalfen W. Conditioned cyclosporine effects but not conditioned taste aversion in immunized rats. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:716. [PubMed: 2244980]
61.
Djuric V, et al. Anaphylactic shock-induced conditioned taste aversion. II. Correlation between taste aversion and indicators of anaphylactic shock. Brain Behav Immun. 1988;2:24. [PubMed: 3179508]
62.
Markovic B, et al. Anaphylactic shock-induced conditioned taste aversion. I. Demonstration of the phenomenon by means of three modes of CS-US presentation. Brain Behav Immun. 1988;2:11. [PubMed: 3179507]
63.
Goebel MU, et al. Behavioral conditioning of immunosuppression is possible in humans. FASEB J. 2002;16:1869. [PubMed: 12468450]
64.
MacQueen G, et al. Pavlovian conditioning of rat mucosal mast cells to secrete rat mast cell protease II. Science. 1989;243:83. [PubMed: 2911721]
65.
Palermo-Neto J, Guimarães R. Pavlovian conditioning of lung anaphylactic response in rats. Life Sci. 2000;68:611. [PubMed: 11205876]
66.
Markovic B, Dimitrijevic M, Jankovic B. Anaphylactic shock in neuropsychoimmunological research. Int J Neurosci. 1992;67:271. [PubMed: 1305638]
67.
Djuric VJ, et al. Neuroimmune Networks: Physiology and Disease. Wiley-Liss; New York: 1989. Pavlovian conditioning in rats subjected to anaphylactic shock; p. 207.
68.
Djuric VJ, et al. Manifestations of repeated anaphylactic shock are context-specific. Neuroendocrinol Lett. 1987;9:215.
69.
Costa-Pinto FA, et al. Avoidance behavior and neural correlates of allergen exposure in a murine model of asthma. Brain Behav Immun. 2005;19:52. [PubMed: 15581738]
70.
Bermúdez-Rattoni F. Molecular mechanisms of taste-recognition memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:209. [PubMed: 14976520]
71.
Eikelboom R, Stewart J. Conditioning of drug-induced physiological responses. Psychol Rev. 1982;89:507. [PubMed: 7178331]
72.
Pacheco-Lopez G, et al. Behaviorally conditioned enhancement of immune responses. In: Ader R, editor. Psychoneuroimmunology. 4. Academic Press; New York: 2006. p. 631.
73.
Blalock J. The immune system as the sixth sense. J Intern Med. 2005;257:126. [PubMed: 15656872]
74.
Goehler L, et al. Vagal immune-to-brain communication: a visceral chemosensory pathway. Auton Neurosci. 2000;85:49. [PubMed: 11189026]
75.
Domjan M. Role of ingestion in odortoxicosis learning in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1973;84:507. [PubMed: 4745819]
76.
Berman D, Dudai Y. Memory extinction, learning anew, and learning the new: dissociations in the molecular machinery of learning in cortex. Science. 2001;291:2417. [PubMed: 11264539]
77.
Mignini F, Streccioni V, Amenta F. Autonomic innervation of immune organs and neuroimmune modulation. Auton Autacoid Pharmacol. 2003;23:1. [PubMed: 14565534]
78.
Elenkov I, et al. The sympathetic nerve: an integrative interface between two super-systems: the brain and the immune system. Pharmacol Rev. 2000;52:595. [PubMed: 11121511]
79.
Sanders V, Kohm A. Sympathetic nervous system interaction with the immune system. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2002;52:17. [PubMed: 12498099]
80.
Sanders V, Straub R. Norepinephrine, the beta-adrenergic receptor, and immunity. Brain Behav Immun. 2002;16:290. [PubMed: 12096881]
81.
Artico M, et al. Noradrenergic and cholinergic innervation of the bone marrow. Int J Mol Med. 2002;10:77. [PubMed: 12060854]
82.
Miyan J, Broome C, Whetton A. Neural regulation of bone marrow. Blood. 1998;92:2971. [PubMed: 9763586]
83.
Maestroni G. Catecholaminergic regulation of hematopoiesis in mice. Blood. 1998;92:2971. 2972. author reply: [PubMed: 9763585]
84.
Downing J, Miyan J. Neural immunoregulation: emerging roles for nerves in immune homeostasis and disease. Immunol Today. 2000;21:281. [PubMed: 10825740]
85.
Hori T, et al. The autonomic nervous system as a communication channel between the brain and the immune system. Neuroimmunomodulation. 1995;2:203. [PubMed: 8963749]
86.
Katafuchi T, et al. Hypothalamic modulation of splenic natural killer cell activity in rats. J Physiol. 1993;471:209. [PMC free article: PMC1143959] [PubMed: 8120804]
87.
Katafuchi T, Take S, Hori T. Roles of sympathetic nervous system in the suppression of cytotoxicity of splenic natural killer cells in the rat. J Physiol. 1993;465:343. [PMC free article: PMC1175433] [PubMed: 8229839]
88.
Pavlov V, Tracey K. The cholinergic anti-inflammatory pathway. Brain Behav Immun. 2005 [PubMed: 15922555]
89.
Czura C, Tracey K. Autonomic neural regulation of immunity. J Intern Med. 2005;257:156. [PubMed: 15656874]
90.
Garcia J, Kimeldorf DJ, Koelling RA. Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science. 1955;122:157. [PubMed: 14396377]
91.
Kawai R, et al. Conditioned taste aversion with sucrose and tactile stimuli in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82:164. [PubMed: 15341802]
92.
Marella S, et al. Imaging taste responses in the fly brain reveals a functional map of taste category and behavior. Neuron. 2006;49:285. [PubMed: 16423701]
93.
Paradis S, Cabanac M. Flavor aversion learning induced by lithium chloride in reptiles but not in amphibians. Behav Proc. 2004;67:11. [PubMed: 15182921]
94.
Schedlowski M. Insecta immune-cognitive interactions. Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20:133. [PubMed: 16260114]
95.
Garb JL, Stunkard AJ. Taste aversions in man. Am J Psychiatry. 1974;131:1204. [PubMed: 4419019]
96.
Sewards TV. Dual separate pathways for sensory and hedonic aspects of taste. Brain Res Bull. 2004;62:271. [PubMed: 14709342]
97.
Sewards TV, Sewards M. Separate, parallel sensory and hedonic pathways in the mammalian somatosensory system. Brain Res Bull. 2002;58:243. [PubMed: 12128150]
98.
Yamamoto T, et al. Neural substrates for conditioned taste aversion in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 1994;65:123. [PubMed: 7718144]
99.
Bermudez-Rattoni F, McGaugh JL. Insular cortex and amygdala lesions differentially affect acquisition on inhibitory avoidance and conditioned taste aversion. Brain Res. 1991;549:165. [PubMed: 1654172]
100.
Cubero I, Thiele TE, Bernstein IL. Insular cortex lesions and taste aversion learning: effects of conditioning method and timing of lesion. Brain Res. 1999;839:323. [PubMed: 10519056]
101.
Sewards TV, Sewards MA. Cortical association areas in the gustatory system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:395. [PubMed: 11566478]
102.
Chen J, et al. Enhancement of antibody production and expression of c-Fos in the insular cortex in response to a conditioned stimulus after a single-trial learning paradigm. Behav Brain Res. 2004;154:557. [PubMed: 15313045]
103.
Ramírez-Amaya V, Bermúdez-Rattoni F. Conditioned enhancement of antibody production is disrupted by insular cortex and amygdala but not hippocampal lesions. Brain Behav Immun. 1999;13:46. [PubMed: 10371677]
104.
Reilly S, Bornovalova MA. Conditioned taste aversion and amygdala lesions in the rat: a critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:1067. [PubMed: 15893375]
105.
Swanson LW, Petrovich GD. What is the amygdala? Trends Neurosci. 1998;21:323. [PubMed: 9720596]
106.
Ramírez-Amaya V, et al. Insular cortex lesions impair the acquisition of conditioned immunosuppression. Brain Behav Immun. 1996;10:103. [PubMed: 8811934]
107.
Ramírez-Amaya V, Alvarez-Borda B, Bermúdez-Rattoni F. Differential effects of NMDA-induced lesions into the insular cortex and amygdala on the acquisition and evocation of conditioned immunosuppression. Brain Behav Immun. 1998;12:149. [PubMed: 9646939]
108.
Vettor R, et al. Neuroendocrine regulation of eating behavior. J Endocrinol Invest. 2002;25:836. [PubMed: 12508946]
109.
Saito M, Minokoshi Y, Shimazu T. Accelerated norepinephrine turnover in peripheral tissues after ventromedial hypothalamic stimulation in rats. Brain Res. 1989;481:298. [PubMed: 2566357]
110.
Okamoto S, et al. Ventromedial hypothalamus suppresses splenic lymphocyte activity through sympathetic innervation. Brain Res. 1996;739:308. [PubMed: 8955952]
111.
Pacheco-López G, et al. Neural substrates for behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression in the rat. J Neurosci. 2005;25:2330. [PMC free article: PMC6726099] [PubMed: 15745959]
112.
Hsueh C, et al. Involvement of catecholamines in recall of the conditioned NK cell response. J Neuroimmunol. 1999;94:172. [PubMed: 10376951]
113.
Kuo J, et al. The involvement of glutamate in recall of the conditioned NK cell response. J Neuroimmunol. 2001;118:245. [PubMed: 11498259]
114.
Hsueh C, et al. Cholinergic and serotonergic activities are required in triggering conditioned NK cell response. J Neuroimmunol. 2002;123:102. [PubMed: 11880155]
115.
Balschun D, et al. Interleukin-6: a cytokine to forget. FASEB J. 2004;18:1788. [PubMed: 15345694]
116.
Tonelli LH, Postolache TT, Sternberg EM. Inflammatory genes and neural activity: involvement of immune genes in synaptic function and behavior. Front Biosci. 2005;10:675. [PubMed: 15569608]
117.
Gibertini M. IL1 beta impairs relational but not procedural rodent learning in a water maze task. Adv Exp Med Biol. 1996;402:207. [PubMed: 8787662]
118.
Schneider H, et al. A neuromodulatory role of interleukin-1-beta in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:7778. [PMC free article: PMC22755] [PubMed: 9636227]
119.
Fiore M, et al. Learning performances, brain NGF distribution and NPY levels in transgenic mice expressing TNF-alpha. Behav Brain Res. 2000;112:165. [PubMed: 10862948]
120.
Banks WA, et al. Intravenous human interleukin-1alpha impairs memory processing in mice: dependence on blood-brain barrier transport into posterior division of the septum. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2001;299:536. [PubMed: 11602664]
121.
Matsumoto Y, et al. Effects of intrahippocampal CT105, a carboxyl terminal fragment of beta-amyloid precursor protein, alone/with inflammatory cytokines on working memory in rats. J Neurochem. 2002;82:234. [PubMed: 12124424]
122.
Matsumoto Y, et al. Involvement of cholinergic and glutamatergic functions in working memory impairment induced by interleukin-1-beta in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 2001;430:283. [PubMed: 11711044]
123.
Rachal Pugh C, et al. The immune system and memory consolidation: a role for the cytokine IL-1-beta. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:29. [PubMed: 11166076]
124.
Lynch MA. Interleukin-1 beta exerts a myriad of effects in the brain and in particular in the hippocampus: analysis of some of these actions. Vitam Horm. 2002;64:185. [PubMed: 11898392]
125.
Besedovsky HO, del Rey A. Immune-neuroendocrine interactions: facts and hypotheses. Endocr Rev. 1996;17:64. [PubMed: 8641224]
126.
Turnbull AV, Rivier CL. Regulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis by cytokines: actions and mechanisms of action. Physiol Rev. 1999;79:1. [PubMed: 9922367]
127.
Sredni-Kenigsbuch D. TH1/TH2 cytokines in the central nervous system. Int J Neurosci. 2002;112:665. [PubMed: 12325311]
128.
Szelényi J. Cytokines and the central nervous system. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54:329. [PubMed: 11306183]
129.
Del Rey A, et al. Not all peripheral immune stimuli that activate the HPA axis induce proinflammatory cytokine gene expression in the hypothalamus. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2000;917:169. [PubMed: 11268341]
130.
Pitossi F, et al. Induction of cytokine transcripts in the central nervous system and pituitary following peripheral administration of endotoxin to mice. J Neurosci Res. 1997;48:287. [PubMed: 9169855]
131.
Tazi A, et al. Interleukin-1 induces conditioned taste aversion in rats: a possible explanation for its pituitary-adrenal stimulating activity. Brain Res. 1988;473:369. [PubMed: 3266091]
132.
Dyck DG, et al. The Pavlovian conditioning of IL-1-induced glucocorticoid secretion. Brain Behav Immun. 1990;4:93. [PubMed: 2168227]
133.
Janz LJ, et al. Conditioned taste aversion but not adrenal activity develops to ICV administration of interleukin-1 in rats. Physiol Behav. 1991;49:691. [PubMed: 1652773]
134.
Hiramoto R, et al. Identification of specific pathways of communication between the CNS and NK cell system. Life Sci. 1993;53:527. [PubMed: 8350666]
135.
Tada T. The immune system as a supersystem. Annu Rev Immunol. 1997;15:1. [PubMed: 9143679]
136.
Ottaviani E, Valensin S, Franceschi C. The neuro-immunological interface in an evolutionary perspective: the dynamic relationship between effector and recognition systems. Front Biosci. 1998;3:431. [PubMed: 9545439]
137.
Salzet M, Vieau D, Day R. Cross-talk between nervous and immune systems through the animal kingdom: focus on opioids. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:550. [PubMed: 11074264]
138.
Vishwanath R. The psychoneuroimmunological system: a recently evolved networking organ system. Med Hypotheses. 1996;47:265. [PubMed: 8910873]
139.
Ottaviani E, Franceschi C. The neuroimmunology of stress from invertebrates to man. Prog Neurobiol. 1996;48:421. [PubMed: 8804115]
140.
Straub R, Besedovsky H. Integrated evolutionary, immunological, and neuroendocrine framework for the pathogenesis of chronic disabling inflammatory diseases. FASEB J. 2003;17:2176. [PubMed: 14656978]
141.
Clatworthy A. Neural-immune interactions: an evolutionary perspective. Neuroimmunomodulation. 1998;5:136. [PubMed: 9730679]
142.
Mallon E, Brockmann A, Schmid-Hempel P. Immune response inhibits associative learning in insects. Proc Biol Sci. 2003;270:2471. [PMC free article: PMC1691534] [PubMed: 14667337]
143.
Riddell CE, Mallon EB. Insect psychoneuroimmunology: immune response reduces learning in protein-starved bumblebees (Bombus terrestris). Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20:135. [PubMed: 16084688]
144.
Rescorla R. Contemporary study of Pavlovian conditioning. Span J Psychol. 2003;6:185. [PubMed: 14628705]
145.
Pinto A, et al. Conditioned enhancement of cough response in awake guinea pigs. Int Arch Allergy Immunol. 1995;108:95. [PubMed: 7647591]
146.
Russell M, et al. Learned histamine release. Science. 1984;225:733. [PubMed: 6205449]
147.
Irie M, Maeda M, Nagata S. Can conditioned histamine release occur under urethane anesthesia in guinea pigs? Physiol Behav. 2001;72:567. [PubMed: 11282141]
148.
Ader R, et al. Conditioned enhancement of antibody production using antigen as the unconditioned stimulus. Brain Behav Immun. 1993;7:334. [PubMed: 8280926]
149.
Alvarez-Borda B, et al. Enhancement of antibody production by a learning paradigm. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1995;64:103. [PubMed: 7582817]
150.
MacKenzie J. The production of the so-called rose effect by means of an artificial rose, with remarks and historical notes. Am J Med Sci. 1886;91:45.
151.
Ikemi Y, Nakagawa S. A psychosomatic study of contagious dermatitis. Kyushu J Med Sci. 1962;13:335.
152.
Dekker E, Pelser H, Groen J. Conditioning as a cause of asthmatic attacks: a laboratory study. J Psychosom Res. 1957;2:97. [PubMed: 13449901]
153.
Turnbull J. Asthma conceived as a learned response. J Psychosom Res. 1962;6:59. [PubMed: 13923184]
154.
Gauci M, et al. Pavlovian conditioning of nasal tryptase release in human subjects with allergic rhinitis. Physiol Behav. 1994;55:823. [PubMed: 8022900]
155.
Smith GR, McDaniel SM. Psychologically mediated effect on the delayed hypersensitivity reaction to tuberculin in humans. Psychosom Med. 1983;45:65. [PubMed: 6844530]
156.
Booth RJ, Petrie KJ, Brook RJ. Conditioning allergic skin responses in humans: a controlled trial. Psychosom Med. 1995;57:492. [PubMed: 8552741]
157.
Bovbjerg DH, et al. Anticipatory immune suppression and nausea in women receiving cyclic chemotherapy for ovarian cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1990;58:153. [PubMed: 2335631]
158.
Lekander M, et al. Anticipatory immune changes in women treated with chemotherapy for ovarian cancer. Int J Behav Med. 1995;2:1. [PubMed: 16250785]
159.
Stockhorst U, et al. Anticipatory symptoms and anticipatory immune responses in pediatric cancer patients receiving chemotherapy: features of a classically conditioned response? Brain Behav Immun. 2000;14:198. [PubMed: 10970680]
160.
Andrykowski MA. Defining anticipatory nausea and vomiting: differences among cancer chemotherapy patients who report pretreatment nausea. J Behav Med. 1988;11:59. [PubMed: 3367372]
161.
Matteson S, et al. The role of behavioral conditioning in the development of nausea. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2002;186:S239. [PubMed: 12011893]
162.
Morrow GR, Lindke J, Black PM. Anticipatory nausea development in cancer patients: replication and extension of a learning model. Br J Psychol. 1991;82 (Pt 1):61. [PubMed: 2029605]
163.
DiLorenzo TA, et al. Sources of anticipatory emotional distress in women receiving chemotherapy for breast cancer. Ann Oncol. 1995;6:705. [PubMed: 8664193]
164.
Jacobsen PB, Bovbjerg DH, Redd WH. Anticipatory anxiety in women receiving chemotherapy for breast cancer. Health Psychol. 1993;12:469. [PubMed: 8293730]
165.
Bovbjerg DH, Montgomery GH, Raptis G. Evidence for classically conditioned fatigue responses in patients receiving chemotherapy treatment for breast cancer. J Behav Med. 2005;28:231. [PubMed: 16015457]
166.
Redd WH, et al. Nausea induced by mental images of chemotherapy. Cancer. 1993;72:629. [PubMed: 8319197]
167.
Dadds MR, et al. Imagery in human classical conditioning. Psychol Bull. 1997;122:89. [PubMed: 9204782]
168.
Benschop RJ, Rodriguez-Feuerhahn M, Schedlowski M. Catecholamine-induced leukocytosis: early observations, current research, and future directions. Brain Behav Immun. 1996;10:77. [PubMed: 8811932]
169.
Schedlowski M, et al. Catecholamines modulate human NK cell circulation and function via spleen-independent beta 2-adrenergic mechanisms. J Immunol. 1996;156:93. [PubMed: 8598500]
170.
Buske-Kirschbaum A, et al. Conditioned increase of natural killer cell activity (NKCA) in humans. Psychosom Med. 1992;54:123. [PubMed: 1373508]
171.
Buske-Kirschbaum A, et al. Conditioned manipulation of natural killer (NK) cells in humans using a discriminative learning protocol. Biol Psychol. 1994;38:143. [PubMed: 7873699]
172.
Kirschbaum C, et al. Conditioning of drug-induced immunomodulation in human volunteers: a European collaborative study. Br J Clin Psychol. 1992;31 (Pt 4):459. [PubMed: 1483156]
173.
Giang DW, et al. Conditioning of cyclophosphamide-induced leukopenia in humans. J Neuropsychiatr Clin Neurosci. 1996;8:194. [PubMed: 9081556]
174.
Longo DL, et al. Conditioned immune response to interferon-[gamma] in humans. Clin Immunol. 1999;90:173. [PubMed: 10080828]
175.
Goebel M, et al. Behavioral conditioning with interferon beta-1a in humans. Physiol Behav. 2005;84:807. [PubMed: 15885259]
176.
Bukrinsky M. Cyclophilins: unexpected messengers in intercellular communications. Trends Immunol. 2002;23:323. [PubMed: 12103338]
177.
McEwen B, Lasley E. Allostatic load: when protection gives way to damage. Adv Mind Body Med. 2003;19:28. [PubMed: 12825411]
178.
McEwen B. Stress, adaptation, and disease: allostasis and allostatic load. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1998;840:33. [PubMed: 9629234]
179.
Ader R. The role of conditioning in pharmacotherapy. In: Harrington A, editor. The Placebo Effect: An Interdisciplinary Exploration. Harvard University Press; Cambridge: 1997. p. 138.
180.
Pacheco-López G, Niemi MB, Schedlowski M. 2006 unpublished data.
Copyright © 2007, Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
Bookshelf ID: NBK3908PMID: 21204427

Views

  • PubReader
  • Print View
  • Cite this Page

Other titles in this collection

Related information

  • PMC
    PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed
    Links to PubMed

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...