U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Duncan JR, Byard RW, editors. SIDS Sudden Infant and Early Childhood Death: The Past, the Present and the Future. Adelaide (AU): University of Adelaide Press; 2018 May.

Cover of SIDS Sudden Infant and Early Childhood Death

SIDS Sudden Infant and Early Childhood Death: The Past, the Present and the Future.

Show details

Chapter 22Autonomic Cardiorespiratory Physiology and Arousal of the Fetus and Infant

, PhD, DSc.

Author Information and Affiliations

Introduction

Despite intensive research over the past decades, the mechanisms which lead to sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) still remain elusive. SIDS is presumed to occur in an apparently healthy infant during a period of sleep (1). A failure of cardiorespiratory control mechanisms, together with an impaired arousal from sleep response, are believed to play an important role in the final event of SIDS. Sleep has a marked influence on respiratory and cardiovascular control in both adults and infants, although sleep states, sleep architecture, and arousal from sleep processes in infants are very different from those of adults and undergo significant maturation during the first year of life, particularly in the first six months when SIDS risk is greatest (2).

Arousal from sleep involves both physiological and behavioral responses and has long been considered a vital survival response for restoring homeostasis in reaction to various life-threatening situations, such as prolonged hypoxia or hypotension (3). There are two distinct arousal types defined in infants, subcortical activation and full cortical arousal, which reflect the hierarchical activation from the brainstem (including heart rate, blood pressure, and ventilation changes) to the cortex (4). Any impairment of these protective responses may render an infant vulnerable to the respiratory and cardiovascular instabilities that are common during infancy and that have been postulated to occur in SIDS. In support of this possibility, extensive physiological and neuropathological studies have provided compelling evidence that impaired cardiovascular control, with a concomitant failure to arouse from sleep, are involved in the final events leading to SIDS. The first six months of life are a critical period of development when rapid maturation of the brain, cardiorespiratory system, and sleep state organization are all taking place (2, 5). Thus, the investigation of sleep physiology in healthy infants during this high-risk period provides important insights into the likely mechanisms involved in the pathogenesis of SIDS.

Development of Sleep

The maturation of sleep is one of the most important physiological processes occurring during the first year of life and is particularly rapid during the first six months after birth (5). Behavioral states in infants are defined by physiological and behavioral variables that are stable over time and occur repeatedly in an individual infant and also across infants (6). The emergence of sleep states is dependent on the central nervous system and is a good and reliable indicator of normal and abnormal development (7).

Sleep states and sleep architecture in infants are quite different from those in adults. In adults, the definition of the two sleep states, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, requires the recording of brain activity (electroencephalogram or EEG), muscle tone (submental electromyogram or EMG), and eye movements (electro-occulogram or EOG). In infants, sleep states are defined using both electrophysiological and behavioral criteria as active sleep (AS) and quiet sleep (QS), which are the precursors of adult REM sleep and NREM sleep, respectively. QS is characterized by high-voltage, low-amplitude EEG activity, the absence of eye movements, and regular heart rate and respiration. In contrast, AS is characterized by low-amplitude, high-frequency EEG activity, eye movements, reduced EMG, and irregular heart rate and respiration (Figure 22.1). In addition, a third state, that of indeterminate sleep (IS), is defined when criteria for AS and QS are not met. IS is usually considered a sign of immaturity and the incidence decreases with increasing postnatal age.

Figure 22.1:. Cardiorespiratory parameters in active and quiet sleep in an infant. ECG: electrocardiograph. EOG: electrooculogram. EMG: electromyogram. EEG: electroencephalogram. BP: blood pressure. RESP ABDO: abdominal respiratory effort. RESP THOR: thoracic respiratory effort. SpO2: oxygen saturation. HR: heart rate. Note regular breathing and heart rate in quiet sleep compared to active sleep. (Author’s own work.).

Figure 22.1:

Cardiorespiratory parameters in active and quiet sleep in an infant. ECG: electrocardiograph. EOG: electrooculogram. EMG: electromyogram. EEG: electroencephalogram. BP: blood pressure. RESP ABDO: abdominal respiratory effort. RESP THOR: thoracic respiratory (more...)

Rhythmic cyclical rest activity patterns can be observed in the human fetus from 28 weeks of gestation (8). In infants born preterm the infant sleep states cannot be distinguished in infants younger than 26 weeks of gestation (9). By 28-30 weeks of gestation AS can be recognized by the presence of eye movements, body movements, and irregular breathing and heart rate. At this gestational age QS is difficult to identify, as submental hypotonia is difficult to evaluate, and the majority of the sleep period is spent in AS. QS does not become clearly identifiable until about 36 weeks of gestational age (8). The percentage of time spent in QS increases with gestational age and by term equal amounts of time are spent in both AS and QS, with the two states alternating throughout each sleep period. The proportion of AS decreases across the first six months of life to make up approximately 25% of total sleep time, similar to that in adults (10). In contrast, the proportion of QS increases with age to make up about 75% of total sleep time by 6 months of age (10).

At term, infants sleep for about 16-17 hours out of every 24 (8). There is a gradual decrease in total sleep time, with infants sleeping 14-15 hours at 16 months of age and 13 to 14 hours by 6 to 8 months of age. In the neonatal period, infants awaken every two to six hours for feeding, regardless of the time of day and stay awake for one to two hours (11). The major change in sleep-wake pattern occurs between six weeks and three months post-term age (11). During the first six months after term, consolidation and entrainment of sleep at night develops and sleep periods lengthen. At 3 weeks of age the mean length of the longest sleep period has been reported to be 211.7 minutes, increasing to 358.0 minutes by 6 months of age (12). The longest sleep period was randomly distributed between daytime and night time at 3 months but had moved to night time by 6 months (12).

Dramatic changes in the sleep EEG patterns of infants occur during early infancy as the brain matures. The EEG patterns of QS and AS differ with a relatively continuous pattern in AS and a relatively discontinuous pattern in QS. A continuous pattern is defined by the presence of background activity throughout each 30-second epoch scored, while a discontinuous pattern is defined by the presence of higher-amplitude EEG waves during <50% of each epoch (13). A semi-discontinuous EEG pattern of depressions and continuous delta activity during ≤70% of each epoch is called a tracé alternant pattern and can be identified at 32-34 weeks of gestational age (13). This pattern is prominent in preterm infants, but it also occurs in infants born at term and disappears one month after term-equivalent age. Sleep spindles appear coincidentally with the disappearance of tracé alternant (14). True continuous delta frequency does not appear until 8-12 weeks of age, and it is not until this age that adult criteria for determining the stages of NREM sleep can be used (15).

In summary, during infancy sleep is at a lifetime maximum and significant changes, which reflect maturation of the central nervous system, occur in the maturation of sleep. Sleep has a marked effect on cardiorespiratory control. Cardiorespiratory disturbances occur predominantly in AS, so the predominance of AS in early infancy may increase the risk of cardiorespiratory disturbances during this period of development.

Effects of sleep state on cardiorespiratory control

There are marked differences in cardiorespiratory control between the two sleep states (AS and QS) (Table 22.1). Assessment of cardiovascular control is commonly made by studies of heart rate and heart rate patterning or variability (HRV) and blood pressure and its variability (16). The short-term, or high-frequency (HF), variability in heart rate is related to parasympathetic vagal activity, while long-term or low-frequency (LF) variability depends on both sympathetic and parasympathetic branches of the autonomic nervous system. The parasympathetic, or vagal, and sympathetic activities constantly interact; however, under resting conditions vagal tone dominates. Vagal afferent stimulation leads to reflex excitation of vagal efferent activity and inhibition of sympathetic efferent activity, resulting in a decrease in heart rate and blood pressure. The opposite reflex effects are mediated by the stimulation of sympathetic afferent activity. Analysis of the changes in heart rate pattern has been used to assess the state and function of the central oscillators, sympathetic and parasympathetic vagal activity, humoral factors, and the sinus node (16).

Table 22.1:. The effects of sleep state on cardiorespiratory variables in infants.

Table 22.1:

The effects of sleep state on cardiorespiratory variables in infants.

In infants, heart rate is higher in AS compared to QS, and this sleep state difference is evident as early as two to three weeks after birth in term infants (17). HRV is also higher in AS compared to QS, as a result of the predominance of sympathetic activity in AS (17). Sleep state also affects blood pressure, with higher values in AS and increased variability (18). Respiratory control is also affected by sleep state. In infants, respiratory rate decreases from wake to sleep, and breathing is more variable in AS, with short apneas occurring more frequently in this state than in QS (5). In infants the majority of apneas are central, and obstructive events are uncommon.

Periodic breathing is common in newborn infants and decreases with increasing postnatal age. Respiratory instability is more common in preterm infants, and the vast majority of these infants will suffer from recurrent prolonged pauses in breathing associated with arterial desaturation and bradycardia, termed apnea of prematurity. Preterm infants also have significantly more episodes of periodic breathing (defined as three or more sequential apneas lasting more than 3 seconds) than those born at term. This immature pattern of breathing is associated with less severe desaturation than apnea of prematurity, but may be very frequent during sleep. Recent studies have shown that periodic breathing is associated with clinically significant falls in cerebral oxygenation which can persist up to six months post-term corrected age (19). Respiratory rate and variability decrease during the first six months of postnatal life (5). An increased respiratory rate in younger infants may be necessary due to the increased demand for carbon dioxide clearance (due to increased metabolic rate), while permitting the infant to maintain ventilation with minimal effort (20). Attempting to modulate ventilation by changing tidal volume would require increased effort due to the highly pliable ribcage and decreased diaphragmatic efficacy (20).

SIDS Risk Factors and Autonomic Control and Arousal

It is currently believed that SIDS is not due to a single factor but is multifactorial in origin. Over the past four decades several models have been proposed to explain the multifactorial nature of SIDS, including the three interrelated causal spheres of influence model (21), the Quadruple Risk Model (22) and the Triple Risk Model (23). The most widely accepted model is the Triple Risk Model for SIDS, which provides useful means for organizing SIDS knowledge. This model proposes that SIDS occurs when three factors occur simultaneously: [1] a vulnerable infant; [2] a critical developmental period for homeostatic control; and [3] an exogenous stressor (23) (Figure 22.2).

Figure 22.2:. Triple Risk Model for SIDS, illustrating the three overlapping factors: [1] a vulnerable infant; [2] a critical developmental period; and [3] an exogenous stress. (Adapted from (23).).

Figure 22.2:

Triple Risk Model for SIDS, illustrating the three overlapping factors: [1] a vulnerable infant; [2] a critical developmental period; and [3] an exogenous stress. (Adapted from (23).). This model proposes that when a vulnerable infant, such as one born (more...)

In an attempt to understand how the Triple Risk Hypothesis is related to infant cardiorespiratory physiology, many researchers have examined how the known risk and protective factors for SIDS alter infant physiology and arousal, particularly during sleep. The remainder of this chapter discusses the association between the three components of the Triple Risk Hypothesis and major risk factors for SIDS, such as prone sleeping and maternal smoking, together with three “protective” factors and cardiovascular control and arousability from sleep in infants, along with their potential involvement in SIDS.

Vulnerable infant

Neuropathologic findings from SIDS victims show significant deficits in brainstem and cerebellar structures involved in the regulation of both respiratory drive, cardiovascular control, sleep/wake transition, and arousal from sleep (25-32). Furthermore, genetic polymorphisms have been identified in SIDS victims which affect genes involved in autonomic function, neurotransmission, energy metabolism, and the response to infection (33-37). Infants with certain genetic polymorphisms are believed to be more vulnerable to SIDS, particularly when these are associated with challenges caused by suspected suboptimal intrauterine and postnatal environments (24).

Exposure to cigarette smoke, alcohol, and illicit drugs

Prenatal and/or postnatal exposure to cigarette smoke is one factor which increases infant vulnerability to SIDS (38, 39), with over 40 studies showing a positive association with risk ratios of between 0.7 and 4.85 (40-43). This increased SIDS risk is likely to be due to the effects of nicotine exposure on autonomic control and arousal (32, 44-48). In support of this idea, Duncan and colleagues (49) found that chronic exposure to nicotine in the prenatal baboon fetus altered serotonergic and nicotinic acetylcholine receptor binding in regions of the medulla, critical to cardiorespiratory control. Furthermore, they identified that these alterations were associated with abnormalities in fetal heart rate variability, indicating altered cardiovascular control (49). Studies in infants exposed to maternal smoking have demonstrated altered heart rate and blood pressure control compared with control infants (50-56). Maternal tobacco smoking also decreases both total arousability and the proportion of cortical arousals. Arousal impairment was observed for both spontaneous arousals from sleep and responses induced by various stimuli (57-63). Few mothers change their smoking behavior postpartum (64); it is therefore difficult to ascertain whether these physiological effects are caused by prenatal or postnatal smoke exposure. Environmental smoke (in the same room) independently increases the risk of SIDS (65, 66). Importantly, it has been shown that before discharge from hospital, preterm infants of smoking mothers already exhibited disruptions in sleep patterns prior to any postnatal smoke exposure (67).

Exposure to alcohol is also a risk for SIDS (68, 69). Animal studies have shown that arousal latency to hypoxia is increased when rat pups were exposed to prenatal alcohol (70). Furthermore, a number of studies have identified that prenatal exposure to illicit drugs also increases the risk for SIDS (71-73). Studies have shown that infants exposed to illicit drugs had an impaired response to a hypoxic challenge at the peak age for SIDS (74). However, another study found no differences in arousal responses to auditory stimuli in methamphetamine-exposed infants; however, infants in both groups were also exposed to maternal smoking (75).

In summary, there is considerable evidence from both animal and human studies suggesting that prenatal exposure to cigarette smoke, alcohol, and illicit drugs has deleterious effects on the developing brain and cardiorespiratory system. It is suggested that these effects increase infant vulnerability to SIDS.

Preterm birth

Maternal smoking may also be a confounding risk factor for SIDS due to its association with other risk factors, such as preterm birth and intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR) (76-79), which likely result from suboptimal intrauterine environments. Regardless of prenatal exposure to maternal smoking, infants born both preterm and IUGR are at increased risk for SIDS (80-87). The proportion of infants who die suddenly and unexpectedly and who are preterm is approximately four times as great as those born at term (20% compared to 5%). These proportional differences have remained unchanged since the introduction of public campaigns for reducing the risks, and the risk factors for preterm infants are similar to those born at term (84, 85). Despite more than halving the rate for preterm infant deaths attributed to SIDS over the last 20 years, the risk for SIDS deaths among preterm infants compared with infants born at term remains elevated (87).

The risk for SIDS in preterm infants has also been shown to be inversely related to gestational age (88-95), with one study demonstrating that the incidence of SIDS in infants born at 24-28 weeks, 29-32 weeks, 33-36 weeks, and more than 37 weeks was 3.52, 3.01, 2.27, and 1.06 deaths per 1,000 live births respectively (83).

Impaired heart rate control, manifesting as shorter cardiac R-R intervals and higher resting sympathetic tone, has been reported in term-born IUGR infants when compared with infants of appropriate size for gestational age (96, 97). Similarly, preterm infants demonstrated impaired autonomic control compared with term infants studied at, or before, term-equivalent age, and this pattern was inversely related to gestational age at birth (98-103). Longitudinal studies after term-equivalent age have identified that preterm infants exhibited lower blood pressure, delayed blood pressure recovery following head-up tilting, and impaired baroreflex control of blood pressure and heart rate across the first six months corrected age, when compared with age-matched term infants (104-108). Furthermore, maturation of baroreflex control of blood pressure is affected by gestational age at birth, with infants born very preterm (<32 weeks of gestation) having reduced increases in baroreflex sensitivity compared to both preterm and term infants (109). Recently, studies have also identified that cerebral oxygenation is also lower in preterm, compared to term, infants across the first six months corrected age (110) and that cerebrovascular control after a head-up tilt is more variable (111), indicating immature or impaired control.

When compared with term infants at matched conceptional ages, preterm infants also exhibit decreased frequencies and durations of spontaneous arousals from sleep (112-114), together with decreased heart rate responses following arousal (115). Furthermore, preterm infants exhibited longer arousal latencies after exposure to mild hypoxia (15% inspired O2), reaching significantly lower oxygen saturations than term infants (116). Cardiorespiratory complications commonly associated with prematurity, apnea and bradycardia, have also been shown to suppress total arousability when these infants were compared to preterm infants with no history of apnea (117).

In summary, infants born preterm and growth-restricted are at increased risk for SIDS, and alterations in cardiorespiratory control and arousability during sleep likely underpin this vulnerability. Such physiological disturbances may be further exacerbated during a critical developmental period within infancy and by exposure to exogenous stressors.

Critical developmental period

Approximately 90% of SIDS deaths occur in infants aged less than 6 months (24, 118). During this period, the central nervous system undergoes dramatic maturational changes which are reflected in extensive alterations to sleep architecture, EEG characteristics, and autonomic cardiorespiratory control. The 2- to 4-month period, in particular, has been described as a “developmental window of vulnerability” (119, 120), and coincides with the age where a distinct peak in SIDS incidence occurs (24, 118). The age of peak SIDS incidence has been reported to have remained constant at 2 to 4 months of age following the introduction of safe sleeping campaigns in some studies (118, 121). However, other studies have reported that the peak SIDS incidence may now occur at an earlier age (122), with a decrease in the median peak age from 80 to 64 days in Sweden (123) and 91 to 66 days in South West England (124) since the initiation of “Back to Sleep” campaigns.

Autonomic control

Autonomic function increases with gestational age in the fetus during pregnancy (125, 126). Both parasympathetic and sympathetic activities increase during gestation, but not in the same manner. The largest increase in parasympathetic activity occurs during the last trimester, while the largest increase in sympathetic activity occurs early on, with smaller changes occurring during the last trimester (127). After birth at term, heart rate increases initially over the first month of life before declining gradually, as a result of an increase in parasympathetic dominance of autonomic control of heart rate (18, 128). Studies of the maturation of heart rate control using HRV have shown an increasing dominance of parasympathetic control across the first six months of life (17). Preterm birth has been associated with immaturity of autonomic nervous system control of the cardiovascular system. This manifests as higher heart rates (98, 99), reduced heart rate variability (99, 129, 130), and decreased baroreflex sensitivity compared to infants born at term (131, 132). Longitudinal studies examining the maturation of blood pressure and its control during sleep in healthy term infants are limited, primarily because of the difficulty of measuring blood pressure continuously and non-invasively. From large studies using intermittent blood pressure measurements, it has been identified that during the first six weeks after birth, systolic blood pressure rises rapidly to reach a steady level which is maintained during infancy (133). Diastolic blood pressure has been shown to fall after birth, reaching a nadir at approximately 2 months of age, followed by a gradual increase until 1 year of life (133).

A number of significant developmental factors may make an infant more vulnerable to a cardiorespiratory challenge during this critical developmental period from birth to 6 months of age, and particularly between 2 to 4 months when the risk of SIDS peaks. Studies in both preterm (105, 110) and term (18) infants have identified a nadir in basal blood pressure during sleep at 2 to 4 months of age, when compared to both earlier (2 to 4 weeks) and later (5 to 6 months) ages studied; a nadir in physiological anemia also occurs at this age. Blood pressure responses to a cardiovascular challenge (head-up tilting) are also impaired at 2 to 4 months compared to younger (2 to 4 weeks) and older (5 to 6 months) ages (134). Studies have also shown that there is a maturational reduction in cerebral oxygenation, which is most marked between 2 to 4 weeks and 2 to 4 months of age, which may be due to limited or inadequate flow-metabolism coupling at this age (135). Thus, the 2- to 4-month age could represent a critical time period when effects of low blood pressure could accentuate decrements in oxygen-carrying capacity and delivery to critical organs (18).

There are even fewer studies of maturation of blood pressure variability (BPV) in infants. BPV decreases with increasing gestational age until term, suggesting a reduction in sympathetic modulation to term-equivalent age (136). BPV continues to decrease with postnatal age after term (137). Taken together these findings suggest that, while sympathetic vascular control is predominant in the newborn period, there is a shift to that of predominantly parasympathetic control with increasing postnatal age, with critical maturational changes occurring when the risk of SIDS is greatest.

The arterial baroreflex is the most important autonomic regulatory mechanism for the short-term control of blood pressure, heart rate, and cardiac contractility. This reflex minimizes changes in blood pressure primarily by altering heart rate and arterial vascular tone. Thus, when there is an increase in blood pressure, it is countered by a decrease in both heart rate and arterial vascular tone. The responses of heart rate and vascular tone are mediated by the efferent parasympathetic and sympathetic limb of the baroreflex respectively. As both systems are involved, studies of the baroreflex provide information on the sympathovagal balance of control of the autonomic nervous system. The baroreflex is present and functional from early fetal life and undergoes significant maturation in utero. The effectiveness of the baroreflex, termed baroreflex sensitivity, increases significantly with postnatal age from 5 to 6 ms/mmHg at 2 to 4 weeks of age to 11-16 ms/mmHg at 5 to 6 months of age, a value similar to that reported in adults (138). Whilst immature, infants may be at increased vulnerability to hypotensive or hypertensive events.

Effects of postnatal age on respiratory control

In order to maintain blood oxygen levels, adult humans exposed to lowered oxygen levels experience a prompt increase in ventilation that peaks within 3 to 5 minutes. This period of hyperventilation is sustained for approximately 15 to 30 minutes before a subsequent decline to pre-hypoxic baseline values. This response is referred to as the hypoxic ventilatory response (HVR). The HVR in infants is quite different to that of adults. Following exposure to low oxygen levels, infants exhibit a “biphasic” HVR response which typically consists of a transient hyperventilation (within the first 2 minutes) termed the augmented phase, followed by a sustained reduction in ventilation towards or below normoxic levels, termed the depressive phase. This biphasic HVR has been demonstrated in term and preterm infants during both wakefulness and sleep and some studies have observed the immature HVR in human infants up to two months after birth (139, 140), while others have shown that it remains immature up to 6 months of age (141).

Arousal responses

Infant arousal responses are also affected by postnatal age, although these maturational effects are sleep-state-dependent. Previous studies have demonstrated that in response to respiratory (mild hypoxia), tactile (nasal air-jet) and auditory stimulation, total arousability is reduced with increasing age during quiet sleep, whilst remaining unchanged in active sleep (142-144). Following the introduction of standard scoring criteria for sub-cortical activation and cortical arousal as separate entities, a recent study noted that spontaneous sub-cortical activations decreased with increasing postnatal age, whilst cortical arousals increased (145). Conversely, another study analyzed both spontaneous and nasal air-jet induced arousability during supine sleep, and found no change in the percentage of cortical arousals (from total responses) throughout the first six months of life (146). Interestingly, when the same infants slept in the prone position, an increased propensity of cortical arousal was identified at 2 to 3 months, the age when SIDS is most common (63, 146). This increase in cortical arousals may reflect an innate protective response to ensure an appropriate level of arousal for restoring homeostasis, not only during a vulnerable period of development, but also in the presence of an exogenous stressor (e.g. the prone sleeping position).

In summary, a large body of work has demonstrated that both autonomic control of the cardiovascular system and arousal responses from sleep are maximally affected by the major risk factors for SIDS, at 2 to 4 months of age when SIDS risk is greatest.

Exogenous stressor(s)

An exogenous stressor constitutes the third aspect of the Triple Risk Model for SIDS. Epidemiological studies have identified numerous factors common to SIDS victims, such as the prone sleeping position, overheating, and recent infection, which may disrupt homeostasis (24, 33, 147, 148).

Prone sleeping

The prone sleeping position has long been considered the major risk factor for SIDS (78, 149-152), with some studies suggesting a causal relation between prone sleep and SIDS (153, 154). Several physiological changes ensue when infants sleep prone, including increased peripheral skin temperature and baseline heart rate, together with decreased heart rate variability (18, 155-163). In an effort to identify changes in autonomic cardiovascular control with sleeping position, studies examining heart rate responses to auditory and nasal air-jet stimuli have suggested an increase in sympathetic, and a decrease in parasympathetic, tone in the prone sleeping position (164, 165). Furthermore, sympathetic effects on blood pressure and vasomotor tone are decreased in the prone sleeping position. Lower resting blood pressure and altered cardiovascular responses to head-up tilting have also been identified in term infants when sleeping in the prone position, compared with the supine position (18, 134). Cerebral oxygenation is reduced and cerebrovascular control impaired in the prone position in both term (135, 166) and preterm infants (110, 167). In addition, prone sleeping infants exhibit reduced cardiac and respiratory responses when arousing from sleep, when compared to sleeping in the supine position (164, 165).

Previous studies of both term and preterm infants have consistently identified increases in sleep time, with significant reductions in spontaneous arousability associated with prone sleeping when compared with the supine position (168-171). Furthermore, in other studies the prone sleeping position depressed arousal responses provoked by postural change (155), auditory (172), and somatosensory challenges (62, 156, 173). It has been demonstrated that both spontaneous and induced arousal responses are similarly affected by sleep state and SIDS risk factors, suggesting that they are mediated through the same pathways (174). Despite this well-documented decrease in total arousability, examining subcortical and cortical responses separately has produced conflicting results. Although one study reported a decreased frequency of spontaneous cortical arousals in the prone position (171), more recent studies have found an increased proportion of cortical arousals (of total responses) in both infants not exposed to cigarette smoke and those exposed to cigarette smoke when sleeping prone (63, 146). This apparent promotion of full cortical arousal, demonstrated for both spontaneous and stimulus-induced responses, may protectively compensate against the threat of altered autonomic control and the already blunted total arousability imposed by the prone position.

The prone sleeping position also potentiates the risk of overheating, by reducing the exposed surface area available for radiant heat loss and reducing respiratory heat loss when the infants face is covered (175). Both physiological studies in healthy infants and theoretical model studies of heat balance have observed a decreased ability to lose heat when in the prone position (176-178). Early studies observed decreased variation in behavior and respiratory pattern, increased heart rate, and increased peripheral skin temperature during prone, compared with supine, sleep (177). These studies suggest that infants are less able to maintain adequate respiratory and metabolic homeostasis when sleeping prone.

Increased sweating occurs in SIDS victims, regardless of whether infants slept prone or supine; these cases were predominantly associated with a covered face (118, 179). A history of profuse sweating in SIDS victims has been postulated to be a phenomenon representing an abnormality of function of the autonomic nervous system (180). The involvement of thermal stress with SIDS is further supported by the finding of similar odds ratios for both too much and too little bedding (181), and the suggestion that future SIDS victims may have had atypical temperature regulation (182). Infant arousability is also affected by body and room temperature: decreased sleep continuity and increased body movements have been associated with exposure to cooler temperatures (183), whilst infants sleeping in warmer environments (28 °C vs 24 °C) exhibited increased arousal thresholds to auditory stimuli (184). Furthermore, based on studies assessing blood pressure control in infants (157, 162), it has been suggested that in response to the increased peripheral skin temperature when infants sleep prone, thermoregulatory vasodilatation of the peripheral microvasculature occurs, resulting in a decrease in blood pressure and a reduction in vasomotor tone. Recent studies in preterm infants have shown that increased ambient temperature led to significant changes in autonomic control with elevated heart rate and lower heart rate variability compared to thermoneutral or cooler temperature (185).

Head covering

Head covering has been identified as a major risk for SIDS, with between 16-28% of SIDS infants found with their heads covered. Although a causal relationship with SIDS has not been established (186, 187), it appears likely that rebreathing and impaired arousal are involved. It has been suggested that the increased SIDS risk associated with head covering may result from hypoxia and hypercapnia via rebreathing of expired air (186, 188). Head covering in healthy infants has profound effects on autonomic control during sleep (189). Franco and colleagues (189) found that infants sleeping supine with their head covered by a bedsheet exhibited decreased parasympathetic activity, increased sympathetic activity, and increased body temperature when compared with head-uncovered periods. In addition, arousal responses in active sleep were also depressed when the head was covered (190).

Bed sharing

Bed sharing or co-sleeping has also been reported to significantly increase the risk of SIDS, particularly when the mother smokes (118, 124, 191, 192), with more than 50% of SIDS deaths occurring in this situation between 1997 and 2006 (193, 194). There have been few studies investigating the physiology behind this risk factor. In infants from non-smoking families who were studied on successive bed-sharing and solitary-sleeping nights, bed sharing was associated with increased awakenings and transient arousals during slow-wave sleep compared to solitary nights (195). In contrast, another study found that bed-sharing infants spent less time moving and were more likely to have their heads partially or fully covered by bedding than cot-sleeping infants (196). Thus more studies are required to identify the exact physiological changes which occur during bed sharing.

Swaddling

Swaddling, or firm wrapping, is a traditional infant care practice which, according to an extensive historical review, has been used in some form or another by various cultures since medieval times (197). Low incidences of SIDS in populations where swaddling is common has led to the proposal that swaddling may be protective (198, 199) and on this basis a number of SIDS prevention organizations recommend it. Several studies have documented a “tranquil” behavioral state with longer sleep periods in swaddled infants. Therefore, despite a disparity between studies on the risk for SIDS (124, 200, 201), swaddling has become increasingly popular as a soothing technique throughout the world (202, 203).

Swaddling is a common practice in infants throughout the first six months of life, during the period of increased SIDS risk. The duration of swaddling and the age of initiation of the practice vary widely. The average duration of swaddling has been reported to be 35 days in infants in Yunnan Province in China (204) and to be for the entire first year of life in rural Turkish children (205). The age of initiation of swaddling also varies, with reports of commencing swaddling in the first month of life in Mongolia (206) and immediately after birth in Russia (207). Currently it is unclear if swaddling is protective against SIDS or if it is indeed a risk. In the United Kingdom during the mid-1990s, swaddling during the last sleep was more common amongst SIDS infants than age-matched controls (14% vs. 9%); furthermore, a more recent study showed that this difference has since become more marked (19% vs. 6%) (124). However, a recent meta-analysis has concluded that swaddling was not protective and that placing an infant prone when swaddled significantly increased the risk of death (208). That there is no evidence to recommend swaddling as a strategy to reduce the risk of SIDS has now been endorsed by the American Academy of Pediatrics (209).

Studies investigating the effect of swaddling on cardiovascular control are limited. Swaddling elicits a mild increase in respiratory frequency, most likely due to restricted tidal volumes imposed by the firm wrapping (210-212). No significant effects have been documented on baseline heart rate, skin temperature, or oxygen saturation in term infants when swaddled during sleep (211, 213). Studies which compared infants who were routinely swaddled to those who were unused to this practice found that sleep time and heart rate variability were only altered in those infants naïve to swaddling (214). Several studies investigated the effects of swaddling in relation to infant arousability; however, divergent results have been published. The commonly observed decreases in spontaneous movements and startle responses with swaddling are in contrast to effects of other protective factors for SIDS (197, 215). One study reported that when infants were swaddled, fewer startle responses progressed to a full awakening, indicating an inhibition of the cortical arousal process (216). More recent studies reported that swaddled infants exhibited increased arousal thresholds in response to nasal air-jet stimulation. Furthermore, a decreased frequency of full cortical arousals was observed primarily in infants who were unaccustomed to being swaddled, at 3 months of age (211). Spontaneous cortical arousals were also decreased in those infants unaccustomed to being swaddled, at 3 months of age (214).

These arousal differences between routinely swaddled and naïve-to-swaddling infants, only at this age of peak SIDS risk, may explain the contradictory findings of another group which found decreased auditory arousal thresholds in swaddled infants when compared to infants who were free to move (213). The authors attributed these effects of swaddling on arousal to the greater autonomic changes found after auditory stimulation in swaddled conditions (217). As with the other risk factors discussed above, the mechanisms whereby swaddling may increase the risk for SIDS remain unclear, and further research is required.

Other external stressors

Other external stressors, such as infection, fever, and minor respiratory and gastrointestinal illnesses, commonly occur in the days to weeks preceding death of SIDS victims (218-220). Although not identified as an independent risk factor for SIDS, minor infections have been associated with an increased likelihood of SIDS when combined with head covering or prone sleeping (122, 200). In the prone sleeping position, minor infection, in combination with fever, could further exacerbate thermoregulatory effects on peripheral vasculature, which could increase the susceptibility of a hypotensive episode. Thus, hypotension, in combination with a decreased ability to arouse from sleep, which has been documented in term infants immediately following an infection (221), could potentially further impair an infant’s ability to appropriately respond to a life-threatening challenge such as circulatory failure or an asphyxial insult.

In summary, numerous studies have shown that exposure to exogenous stressors which have been associated with increased SIDS risk, such as prone sleeping and head covering, impair autonomic cardiorespiratory control and infant arousal responses from sleep. The mechanism of increased risk for other factors, such as bed sharing and swaddling, is less clear and further research is required.

SIDS-“Protective” Factors and Autonomic Control and Arousal

Some studies have suggested that infant care practices such as breastfeeding, dummy/pacifier use, and immunization decrease the risk of SIDS. These potentially protective factors for SIDS have all been associated with alterations to both cardiovascular autonomic control and arousal responses during sleep. However, results are often inconsistent, and supporting evidence is less extensive than for the risk factors discussed above; thus, these potentially preventative factors remain controversial amongst researchers.

Breastfeeding

Breastfeeding reduces the incidence of SIDS by approximately half (odds ratio (OR) 0.52, 95% CI: 0.46-0.60), even after multivariate analyses accounted for potentially confounding socioeconomic factors (218, 222, 223). This apparent protection may be a biological effect, given that breastfeeding has been associated with a decreased incidence of diarrhea, vomiting, colds, and other infections; in addition, breast milk is rich in antibodies and many micronutrients (218, 224, 225). Only one study has assessed the effects of breastfeeding on the cardiovascular system during sleep in term infants, and this study found that heart rate was significantly lower in breastfed infants when compared with formula-fed infants (226). Although little is known about the effects of breastfeeding compared to formula feeding on cardiovascular control in infants, physiological studies have demonstrated an apparent promotion of arousal from sleep associated with breastfeeding. One study found that breastfed infants spent more time awake during the night, thus requiring more frequent parental visits (227). Another study showed that healthy breastfed infants aroused more readily from active sleep than formula-fed infants in response to nasal air-jet stimulation at 2 to 3 months postnatal age (228). Although there is a general consensus that breastfeeding should be encouraged, the relationship between breastfeeding for SIDS prevention remains unclear.

Dummy/Pacifier use

The finding that use of a dummy/pacifier has a protective effect for SIDS has consistently emerged from epidemiological studies, with significant associations being described for both usage during the final sleep and “dummy ever used” (OR: 0.46. CI 0.36-0.59) (118, 229-234). Studies have suggested that a likely mechanism for this protection against SIDS is increased heart rate variability, which has been demonstrated during sucking periods in term (235, 236) and preterm infants (237). Conversely, dummy sucking has also been shown to have no effect on heart rate, heart rate variability, respiratory frequency, or oxygen saturation in term infants (238, 239). In addition, pacifier sucking has been shown to elicit increases in blood pressure in quietly awake or sleeping term infants (236, 240) and in preterm infants (237). Another potential mechanism for the protective nature of dummy use against SIDS is an enhanced arousability from sleep. However, results of the few studies which have been conducted are conflicting, with one study reporting decreased arousal thresholds to auditory stimulation observed in infants who regularly used a dummy, when compared with those who did not use a dummy (241). In contrast, other studies have reported no effect of dummy use on either the frequency or duration of spontaneous arousals in sleeping infants, when studied both with and without a dummy in the mouth (242, 243). It has also been hypothesized that sucking on a dummy during sleep may assist in maintaining airway patency, thus preventing a pharyngeal vacuum and the consequent sealing of the airway (244, 245). Thus the risk of oropharyngeal obstruction may be reduced due to the forward positioning of the tongue when sucking on a dummy (245). Although epidemiological studies have provided strong support for dummy/pacifier use to be protective for SIDS, the physiological mechanisms responsible for this protection remain uncertain.

Immunization

The peak incidence of SIDS coincides at the time when infants are receiving their first triple antigen vaccinations. In the 1970s there were case reports of infant deaths shortly after the diphtheria-tetanus-pertussis immunization and there were concerns that there was a causal relationship. In 2003 the National Academy of Sciences in the USA reviewed the available data and rejected the idea that there was a causal relationship (246). Additionally, large population case-control studies have found that fewer immunized infants die from sudden and unexpected death in infancy (SUDI), and thus immunization is protective (247-249). A more recent meta-analysis found a multivariate OR for immunization and SUDI to be 0.54 (95% CI: 0.31-0.76); in other words the risk of SUDI is halved in immunized infants (250, 251). There have been limited studies on the physiological benefits of vaccination; however, one study showed that arousal responses and sleep patterns were not affected in the immediate post-vaccination period, despite elevated temperature and heart rate (252).

In summary, factors which have been shown to be protective against SIDS show increases in autonomic control and arousability, as in the case of dummy/pacifier use and breastfeeding, or no change, as in the case of immunization.

Conclusions

Assessment of cardiovascular control and arousal processes during sleep is important in understanding sleep-related pathologies such as SIDS. In otherwise healthy infants, studies have demonstrated impairment of these physiological mechanisms in association with all three aspects of the Triple Risk Model, thus demonstrating the heterogeneous nature of SIDS. Altered cardiovascular and cerebrovascular control, in conjunction with a failure to arouse from sleep, could potentially impair an infant’s ability to appropriately compensate for life-threatening challenges, such as prolonged hypotension or asphyxia during sleep. The concept of a close relationship between SIDS and autonomic dysfunction becomes more compelling with the demonstration of an apparent promotion of arousal from sleep by protective factors for SIDS. Despite successful public awareness campaigns dramatically reducing SIDS rates, this decline in SIDS incidence may have stabilized (253-256). Thus, further research is imperative to elucidate the exact mechanisms involved in the final events of SIDS, allowing identification of “at-risk” infants in the future. The ability to identify these infants would have the potential to increase awareness in both parents and clinicians, whilst minimizing the incidence of SIDS with close monitoring and early intervention.

References

1.
Krous HF, Beckwith JB, Byard RW, Rognum TO, Bajanowski T, Corey T, et al. Sudden infant death syndrome and unclassified sudden infant deaths: A definitional and diagnostic approach. Pediatrics. 2004;114:234-8. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.114.1.234. [PubMed: 15231934]
2.
Curzi-Dascalova L, Peirano P, Morel-Kahn F. Development of sleep states in normal premature and full-term newborns. Dev Psychobiol. 1988;21(5):431-44. https://doi​.org/10.1002/dev.420210503. [PubMed: 3402666]
3.
Phillipson EA, Sullivan CE. Arousal: The forgotten response to respiratory stimuli. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1978;118:807-9. [PubMed: 736352]
4.
International Paediatric Work Group on Arousals. The scoring of arousals in healthy term infants (between the ages of 1 and 6 months). J Sleep Res. 2005;14:37-41. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-2869.2004.00426.x. [PubMed: 15743332]
5.
Gaultier C. Cardiorespiratory adaptation during sleep in infants and children. Pediatr Pulmonol. 1995;19(2):105-17. https://doi​.org/10.1002/ppul.1950190206. [PubMed: 7659466]
6.
Prechtl HF. The behavioral states of the newborn infant (a review). Brain Res. 1974;76(2):185-212. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0006-8993(74)90454-5. [PubMed: 4602352]
7.
Curzi-Dascalova L, Challamel MJ. Neurophysiological basis of sleep development. In: Sleep and breathing in children: A developmental approach. Eds Loughlin GM, Carroll JL, Marcus CL. New York: Marcel Dekker, 2000.
8.
Parmelee AH, Stern E. Development of states in infants. In: Sleep and the maturing nervous system. Eds Clemente CD, Purpura DP, Mayer FE. New York: Academic, 1972. p. 199-228.
9.
Dreyfus-Brisac C. Sleep ontogenesis in early human prematurity from 24 to 27 weeks conceptual age. Dev Psychobiol. 1968;1:162-9. https://doi​.org/10.1002/dev.420010303.
10.
de Weerd AW, van den Bossche RA. The development of sleep during the first months of life. Sleep Med Rev. 2003;7(2):179-91. https://doi​.org/10.1053/smrv.2002.0198. [PubMed: 12628217]
11.
Coons SC, Guilleminault C. Development of sleep-wake patterns and non-rapid eye movement sleep stages during the first six months of life in normal infants. Pediatrics. 1982;69(6):793-8. https://doi​.org/10.1097​/00004583-198211000-00028. [PubMed: 7079046]
12.
Coons S. Development of sleep and wakefulness during the first 6 months of life. Ed Guilleminault C. New York: Raven Press, 1987. p. 17-27.
13.
Curzi-Dascalova L, Mirmiran M. Manual of methods for recording and analysing sleep-wakefulness states in preterm and full term infants. Paris: Les Editions INSERM, 1996.
14.
Metcalf D. The ontogenesis of sleep-awake states from birth to 3 months. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1970;28(4):421. [PubMed: 4191207]
15.
Grigg-Damberger M, Gozal D, Marcus CL, Quan SF, Rosen CL, Chervin RD, et al. The visual scoring of sleep and arousal in infants and children. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3(2):201-40. [PubMed: 17557427]
16.
Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation. 1996;93(5):1043-65. https://doi​.org/10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043. [PubMed: 8598068]
17.
Horne RS. Cardio-respiratory control during sleep in infancy. Paediatr Respir Rev. 2014;15(2):163-9. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.prrv.2013.02.012. [PubMed: 23523390]
18.
Yiallourou SR, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Effects of sleeping position on development of infant cardiovascular control. Arch Dis Child. 2008;93(10):868-72. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.2007.132860. [PubMed: 18456690]
19.
Decima PF, Fyfe KL, Odoi A, Wong FY, Horne RS. The longitudinal effects of persistent periodic breathing on cerebral oxygenation in preterm infants. Sleep Med. 2015;16(6):729-35. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.sleep.2015.02.537. [PubMed: 25959095]
20.
Kerem E. Why do infants and small children breathe faster? Pediatr Pulmonol. 1996;21(1):65-8. https://doi​.org/10.1002​/1099-0496(199601)21​:1<65::AID-PPUL1950210104>3.0.CO;2-R. [PubMed: 8776270]
21.
Emery JL. A way of looking at the causes of crib death. In: Proceedings of the International Research Conference on the Sudden Infant Death Syndrome. Eds Tildon JT, Roeder LM, Steinschneider A. New York, USA: Academic Press, 1983.
22.
Mage DT, Donner M. A unifying theory for SIDS. Int J Pediatr. 2009:1-10. https://doi​.org/10.1155/2009/368270. [PMC free article: PMC2798085] [PubMed: 20049339]
23.
Filiano J, Kinney HC. A perspective on neuropathologic findings in victims of the sudden infant death syndrome: The Triple-Risk Model. Biol Neonate. 1994;65:194-7. https://doi​.org/10.1159/000244052. [PubMed: 8038282]
24.
Moon RY, Horne RSC, Hauck FR. Sudden infant death syndrome. Lancet. 2007;370:1578-87. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0140-6736(07)61662-6. [PubMed: 17980736]
25.
Paterson DS, Trachtenberg FL, Thompson EG, Belliveau RA, Beggs AH, Darnall BA, et al. Multiple serotonergic brainstem abnormalities in sudden infant death syndrome. JAMA. 2006;286(17):2124-32. https://doi​.org/10.1001/jama.296.17.2124. [PubMed: 17077377]
26.
Kinney HC, Cryan JB, Haynes RL, Paterson DS, Haas EA, Mena OJ, et al. Dentate gyrus abnormalities in sudden unexplained death in infants: Morphological marker of underlying brain vulnerability. Acta Neuropathol. 2015;129(1):65-80. https://doi​.org/10.1007​/s00401-014-1357-0. [PMC free article: PMC4282685] [PubMed: 25421424]
27.
Kinney HC, Filiano JJ, Sleeper LA, Mandell F, Valdes-Dapena M, White WF. Decreased muscarinic receptor binding in the arcuate nucleus in sudden infant death syndrome. Science. 1995;269(5229):1446-50. https://doi​.org/10.1126/science.7660131. [PubMed: 7660131]
28.
Panigrahy A, Filiano J, Sleeper LA, Mandell F, Valdes-Dapena M, Krous HF, et al. Decreased serotonergic receptor binding in rhombic lip-derived regions of the medulla oblongata in the sudden infant death syndrome. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2000;59(5):377-84. https://doi​.org/10.1093/jnen/59.5.377. [PubMed: 10888367]
29.
Panigrahy A, Filiano JJ, Sleeper LA, Mandell F, Valdes-Dapena M, Krous HF, et al. Decreased kainate receptor binding in the arcuate nucleus of the sudden infant death syndrome. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1997;56(11):1253-61. https://doi​.org/10.1097​/00005072-199711000-00010. [PubMed: 9370236]
30.
Machaalani R, Say M, Waters KA. Serotinergic receptor 1A in the sudden infant death syndrome brainstem medulla and associations with clinical risk factors. Acta Neuropathologica. 2009;117(3):257-65. https://doi​.org/10.1007​/s00401-008-0468-x. [PubMed: 19052756]
31.
Machaalani R, Waters KA. Neuronal cell death in the sudden infant death syndrome brainstem and associations with risk factors. Brain. 2008;131:218-28. https://doi​.org/10.1093/brain/awm290. [PubMed: 18084013]
32.
Machaalani R, Waters KA. Neurochemical abnormalities in the brainstem of the sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS). Paediatr Respir Rev. 2014;15(4):293-300. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.prrv.2014.09.008. [PubMed: 25304427]
33.
Kinney HC, Thach BT. The sudden infant death syndrome. N Engl J Med. 2009;361(8):795-805. https://doi​.org/10.1056/NEJMra0803836. [PMC free article: PMC3268262] [PubMed: 19692691]
34.
Lavezzi AM, Casale V, Oneda R, Weese-Mayer DE, Matturri L. Sudden infant death syndrome and sudden intrauterine unexplained death: Correlation between hypoplasia of raphe nuclei and serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism. Pediatr Res. 2009;66(1):22-7. https://doi​.org/10.1203/PDR​.0b013e3181a7bb73. [PubMed: 19342987]
35.
Weese-Mayer DE, Ackerman MJ, Marazita ML, Berry-Kravis EM. Sudden infant death syndrome: Review of implicated genetic factors. Am J Med Genet A. 2007;143A:771-88. https://doi​.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.31722. [PubMed: 17340630]
36.
Filonzi L, Magnani C, Lavezzi AM, Rindi G, Parmigiani S, Bevilacqua G, et al. Association of dopamine transporter and monoamine oxidase molecular polymorphisms with sudden infant death syndrome and stillbirth: New insights into the serotonin hypothesis. Neurogenetics. 2009;10:65-72. https://doi​.org/10.1007​/s10048-008-0149-x. [PubMed: 18810510]
37.
Courts C, Madea B. Genetics of the sudden infant death syndrome. Forensic Sci Int. 2010;203(1-3):25-33. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.forsciint.2010.07.008. [PubMed: 20674198]
38.
Mitchell E, Scragg R, Stewart AW, Becroft DMO, Taylor B, Ford RPK, et al. Results from the first year of the New Zealand cot death study. NZ Med J. 1991;104:71-6. [PubMed: 2020450]
39.
Matturri L, Ottaviani G, Lavezzi AM. Maternal smoking and sudden infant death syndrome: Epidemiological study related to pathology. Virchows Arch. 2006;449:697-706. https://doi​.org/10.1007​/s00428-006-0308-0. [PubMed: 17091255]
40.
Anderson HR, Cook DG. Passive smoking and sudden infant death syndrome: Review of the epidemilogical evidence. Thorax. 1997;52:1003-9. https://doi​.org/10.1136/thx.52.11.1003. [PMC free article: PMC1758452] [PubMed: 9487351]
41.
Blair PS, Bensley D, Smith I, Bacon C, Taylor B, Berry J. Smoking and the sudden infant death syndrome: Results from 1993-5 case-control study for confidential inquiry into stillbirths and deaths in infancy. BMJ. 1996;313:195-8. https://doi​.org/10.1136/bmj.313.7051.195. [PMC free article: PMC2351602] [PubMed: 8696194]
42.
Dwyer T, Ponsonby A, Couper D. Tobacco smoke exposure at one month of age and subsequent risk of SIDS — A prospective study. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;149:593-602. https://doi​.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals​.aje.a009857. [PubMed: 10192305]
43.
Haglund B. Cigarette smoking and sudden infant death syndrome: Some salient points in the debate. Acta Paediatrica Supplement. 1993;389:37-9. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.1993.tb12872.x. [PubMed: 8374189]
44.
Machaalani R, Say M, Waters KA. Effects of cigarette smoke exposure on nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunits alpha7 and beta2 in the sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) brainstem. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2011;257(3):396-404. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.taap.2011.09.023. [PubMed: 22000980]
45.
Machaalani R, Ghazavi E, Hinton T, Waters KA, Hennessy A. Cigarette smoking during pregnancy regulates the expression of specific nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) subunits in the human placenta. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2014;276(3):204-12. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.taap.2014.02.015. [PubMed: 24607864]
46.
Lavezzi AM, Mecchia D, Matturri L. Neuropathology of the area postrema in sudden intrauterine and infant death syndromes related to tobacco smoke exposure. Auton Neurosci. 2012;166(1-2):29-34. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.autneu.2011.09.001. [PubMed: 21982783]
47.
Hunt NJ, Russell B, Du MK, Waters KA, Machaalani R. Changes in orexinergic immunoreactivity of the piglet hypothalamus and pons after exposure to chronic postnatal nicotine and intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia. Eur J Neurosci. 2016;43(12):1612-22. https://doi​.org/10.1111/ejn.13246. [PubMed: 27038133]
48.
Vivekanandarajah A, Chan YL, Chen H, Machaalani R. Prenatal cigarette smoke exposure effects on apoptotic and nicotinic acetylcholine receptor expression in the infant mouse brainstem. Neurotoxicology. 2016;53:53-63. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.neuro.2015.12.017. [PubMed: 26746805]
49.
Duncan JR, Garland M, Myers MM, Fifer WP, Yang M, Kinney HC, et al. Prenatal nicotine-exposure alters fetal autonomic activity and medullary neurotransmitter receptors: Implications for sudden infant death syndrome. J Appl Physiol. 2009;107(5):1579-90. https://doi​.org/10.1152/japplphysiol​.91629.2008. [PMC free article: PMC2777800] [PubMed: 19729586]
50.
Browne CA, Colditz PB, Dunster KR. Infant autonomic function is altered by maternal smoking during pregnancy. Early Hum Develop. 2000;59:209-18. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0378-3782(00)00098-0. [PubMed: 10996276]
51.
Dahlstrom A, Ebersjo C, Lundell B. Nicotine in breast milk influences heart rate variability in the infant. Acta Paediatrica. 2008;97(8):1075-9. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.2008.00785.x. [PubMed: 18498428]
52.
Fifer WP, Fingers ST, Youngman M, Gomez-Gribben E, Myers MM. Effects of alcohol and smoking during pregnancy on infant autonomic control. Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51:234-42. https://doi​.org/10.1002/dev.20366. [PMC free article: PMC3312313] [PubMed: 19253344]
53.
Cohen G, Vella S, Jeffery H, Lagercrantz H, Katz-Salamon M. Cardiovascular stress hyperreactivity in babies of smokers and in babies born preterm. Circulation. 2008;118(18):1848-53. https://doi​.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA​.108.783902. [PubMed: 18852367]
54.
Thiriez G, Bouhaddi M, Mourot L, Nobili F, Fortrat JO, Menget A, et al. Heart rate variability in preterm infants and maternal smoking during pregnancy. Clin Auton Res. 2009;19(3):149-56. https://doi​.org/10.1007​/s10286-009-0003-8. [PubMed: 19255805]
55.
Viskari-Lahdeoja S, Hytinantti T, Andersson S, Kirjavainen T. Heart rate and blood pressure control in infants exposed to maternal cigarette smoking. Acta Paediatrica. 2008;97(11):1535-41. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.2008.00966.x. [PubMed: 18691163]
56.
Franco P, Chabanski S, Szliwowski H, Dramaix M, Kahn A. Influence of maternal smoking on autonomic nervous system in healthy infants. Pediatr Res. 2000;47(2):215-20. https://doi​.org/10.1203​/00006450-200002000-00011. [PubMed: 10674349]
57.
Sawnani H, Jackson T, Murphy T, Beckerman R, Simakajornboon N. The effect of maternal smoking on respiratory and arousal patterns in preterm infants during sleep. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2004;169:733-8. https://doi​.org/10.1164/rccm​.200305-692OC. [PubMed: 14684558]
58.
Tirosh E, Libon D, Bader D. The effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on sleep respiratory and arousal patterns in neonates. J Perinatol. 1996;16(6):435-8. [PubMed: 8979180]
59.
Franco P, Groswasser J, Hassid S, Lanquart J, Scaillet S, Kahn A. Prenatal exposure to cigarette smoking is associated with a decrease in arousal in infants. J Pediatr. 1999;135(1):34-8. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(99)70324-0. [PubMed: 10393601]
60.
Chang AB, Wilson SJ, Masters IB, Yuill M, Williams G, Hubbard M. Altered arousal response in infants exposed to cigarette smoke. Arch Dis Child. 2003;88:30-3. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.88.1.30. [PMC free article: PMC1719296] [PubMed: 12495956]
61.
Lewis KW, Bosque EM. Deficient hypoxia awakening response in infants of smoking mothers: Possible relationship to sudden infant death syndrome. J Pediatr. 1995;127(5):691-9. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(95)70155-9. [PubMed: 7472818]
62.
Horne RSC, Ferens D, Watts A-M, Vitkovic J, Andrew S, Cranage SM, et al. Effects of maternal tobacco smoking, sleeping position and sleep state on arousal in healthy term infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2002;87:F100-F5. https://doi​.org/10.1136/fn.87.2.F100. [PMC free article: PMC1721454] [PubMed: 12193515]
63.
Richardson HL, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Maternal smoking impairs arousal patterns in sleeping infants. Sleep. 2009;32(4):515-21. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/32.4.515. [PMC free article: PMC2663655] [PubMed: 19413145]
64.
Johansson A, Halling A, Hermansson G. Indoor and outdoor smoking. Impact on children’s health. Eur J Public Health. 2003;13(1):61-6. https://doi​.org/10.1093/eurpub/13.1.61. [PubMed: 12678316]
65.
Schoendorf KC, Kiely JL. Relationship of sudden infant death syndrome to maternal smoking during and after pregnancy. Pediatrics. 1992;90(6):905-8. [PubMed: 1437432]
66.
Klonoff-Cohen HS, Edelstein SL, Lefkowitz ES, Srinivasan IP, Kaegi D, Chang JC, et al. The effect of passive smoking and tobacco exposure through breast milk on sudden infant death syndrome. JAMA. 1995;273(10):795-8. https://doi​.org/10.1001/jama​.1995.03520340051035. [PubMed: 7861574]
67.
Stephan-Blanchard E, Telliez F, Leke A, Djeddi D, Bach V, Libert J, et al. The influence of in utero exposure to smoking on sleep patterns in preterm neonates. Sleep. 2008;31(12):1683-9. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/31.12.1683. [PMC free article: PMC2603491] [PubMed: 19090324]
68.
Strandberg-Larsen K, Gronboek M, Andersen AM, Andersen PK, Olsen J. Alcohol drinking pattern during pregnancy and risk of infant mortality. Epidemiology. 2009;20(6):884-91. https://doi​.org/10.1097/EDE​.0b013e3181bbd46c. [PubMed: 19797967]
69.
O’Leary CM, Jacoby PJ, Bartu A, D’Antoine H, Bower C. Maternal alcohol use and sudden infant death syndrome and infant mortality excluding SIDS. Pediatrics. 2013;131(3):e770-8. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2012-1907. [PubMed: 23439895]
70.
Sirieix CM, Tobia CM, Schneider RW, Darnall RA. Impaired arousal in rat pups with prenatal alcohol exposure is modulated by GABAergic mechanisms. Physiol Rep. 2015;3(6):e12424. https://doi​.org/10.14814/phy2.12424. [PMC free article: PMC4510626] [PubMed: 26059034]
71.
Rajegowda BK, Kandall SR, Falciglia H. Sudden unexpected death in infants of narcotic-dependent mothers. Early Hum Dev. 1978;2(3):219-25. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0378-3782(78)90026-9. [PubMed: 551926]
72.
Chavez CJ, Ostrea EM Jr., Stryker JC, Smialek Z. Sudden infant death syndrome among infants of drug-dependent mothers. J Pediatr. 1979;95(3):407-9. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(79)80517-X. [PubMed: 469666]
73.
Williams SM, Mitchell EA, Taylor BJ. Are risk factors for sudden infant death syndrome different at night? Arch Dis Child. 2002;87(4):274-8. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.87.4.274. [PMC free article: PMC1763023] [PubMed: 12243991]
74.
Ali K, Rossor T, Bhat R, Wolff K, Hannam S, Rafferty GF, et al. Antenatal substance misuse and smoking and newborn hypoxic challenge response. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2016;101(2):F143-8. https://doi​.org/10.1136​/archdischild-2015-308491. [PubMed: 26290480]
75.
Galland BC, Mitchell EA, Thompson JM, Wouldes T, Group NIS. Auditory evoked arousal responses of 3-month-old infants exposed to methamphetamine in utero: A nap study. Acta Paediatrica. 2013;102(4):424-30. https://doi​.org/10.1111/apa.12136. [PMC free article: PMC5341078] [PubMed: 23253105]
76.
Andriessen P, Koolen AMP, Berendsen RCM, Wijn PFF, ten Broeke EDM, Oei SG, et al. Cardiovascular fluctuations and transfer function analysis in stable preterm infants. Pediatr Res. 2003;53(1):89-97. https://doi​.org/10.1203​/00006450-200301000-00016. [PubMed: 12508086]
77.
Mitchell EA, Ford RPK, Stewart AW, Taylor BJ, Becroft DMO, Thompson JMD, et al. Smoking and the sudden infant death syndrome. Pediatrics. 1993;91:893-6. [PubMed: 8474808]
78.
Brooke H, Gibson A, Tappin D, Brown H. Case control study of sudden infant death syndrome in Scotland, 1992-5. BMJ. 1997;314:1516-20. https://doi​.org/10.1136/bmj​.314.7093.1516. [PMC free article: PMC2126747] [PubMed: 9169398]
79.
Schellscheidt J, Oyen N, Jorch G. Interactions between maternal smoking and other perinatal risk factors for SIDS. Acta Paediatrica. 1997;86:857-63. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.1997.tb08612.x. [PubMed: 9307168]
80.
Bergman AB, Ray CG, Pomeroy MA, Wahl PW, Beckwith JB. Studies of the sudden infant death syndrome in King County, Washington. 3. Epidemiology. Pediatrics. 1972;49(6):860-70. [PubMed: 5041320]
81.
Grether JK, Schulman J. Sudden infant death syndrome and birth weight. J Pediatr. 1989;114:561-7. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(89)80694-8. [PubMed: 2926568]
82.
Adams MM, Rhodes PH, McCarthy BJ. Are race and length of gestation related to age at death in the sudden infant death syndrome? Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 1990;4(3):325-39. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-3016.1990.tb00655.x. [PubMed: 2374750]
83.
Malloy MH, Hoffman HJ. Prematurity, sudden infant death syndrome, and age of death. Pediatrics. 1995;96(3):464-71. [PubMed: 7651779]
84.
Blair PS, Platt MW, Smith IJ, Fleming PJ, & CESDI SUDI Research Group. Sudden infant death syndrome and sleeping position in pre-term and low birth weight infants: An opportunity for targeted intervention. Arch Dis Child. 2006;91:101-6. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.2004.070391. [PMC free article: PMC2082697] [PubMed: 15914498]
85.
Thompson JM, Mitchell EA, & New Zealand Cot Death Study Group. Are the risk factors for SIDS different for preterm and term infants? Arch Dis Child. 2006;91(2):107-11. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.2004.071167. [PMC free article: PMC2082673] [PubMed: 15871984]
86.
Gilbert NL, Fell DB, Joseph KS, Liu S, Leon JA, Sauve R, et al. Temporal trends in sudden infant death syndrome in Canada from 1991 to 2005: Contribution of changes in cause of death assignment practices and in maternal and infant characteristics. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2012;26(2):124-30. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-3016.2011.01248.x. [PMC free article: PMC3321219] [PubMed: 22324498]
87.
Malloy MH. Prematurity and sudden infant death syndrome: United States 2005-2007. J Perinatol. 2013;33(6):470-5. https://doi​.org/10.1038/jp.2012.158. [PubMed: 23288251]
88.
Peterson DR. Sudden, unexpected death in infants. An epidemiologic study. Am J Epidemiol. 1966;84(3):478-82. https://doi​.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals​.aje.a120660. [PubMed: 5954453]
89.
Standfast SJ, Jereb S, Janerich DT. The epidemiology of sudden infant death in upstate New York. JAMA. 1979;241(11):1121-4. https://doi​.org/10.1001/jama​.1979.03290370025021. [PubMed: 762763]
90.
Hoffman HJ, Damus K, Hillman L, Krongrad E. Risk factors for SIDS: Results of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development SIDS cooperative epidemiology study. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988;533:13-30. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1749-6632.1988.tb37230.x. [PubMed: 3048169]
91.
Hoffman HJ, Hillman LS. Epidemiology of the sudden infant death syndrome: Maternal, neonatal, and postneonatal risk factors. Clin Perinatol. 1992;19(4):717-37. [PubMed: 1464187]
92.
Grether JK, Schulman J. Sudden infant death syndrome and birth weight. J Pediatrics. 1989;114:561-7. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(89)80694-8. [PubMed: 2926568]
93.
Malloy MH, Hoffman MA. Prematurity, sudden infant death syndrome and age of death. Pediatrics. 1995;96(3):464-71. [PubMed: 7651779]
94.
Malloy MH, Freeman DH Jr. Birth weight- and gestational age-specific sudden infant death syndrome mortality: United States, 1991 versus 1995. Pediatrics. 2000;105(6):1227-31. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.105.6.1227. [PubMed: 10835061]
95.
Halloran DR, Alexander GR. Preterm delivery and age of SIDS death. Ann Epidemiol. 2006;16(8):600-6. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.annepidem.2005.11.007. [PubMed: 16414275]
96.
Galland BC, Taylor B, Bolton DPG, Sayers RM. Heart rate variability and cardiac reflexes in small for gestational age infants. J Appl Physiol. 2006;100(3):933-9. https://doi​.org/10.1152/japplphysiol​.01275.2005. [PubMed: 16306252]
97.
Spassov L, Curzi-Dascalova L, Clairambault J, Kauffmann F, Eiselt M, Medigue C, et al. Heart rate and heart rate variability during sleep in small-for-gestational age newborns. Pediatr Res. 1994;35(4 Pt 1):500-5. https://doi​.org/10.1203​/00006450-199404000-00022. [PubMed: 8047389]
98.
Katona PG, Frasz A, Egbert J. Maturation of cardiac control in full-term and preterm infants during sleep. Early Hum Dev. 1980;4(2):145-59. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0378-3782(80)90018-3. [PubMed: 7408745]
99.
Eiselt M, Curzi-Dascalova L, Clairambault J, Kauffmann F, Medigue C, Peirano P. Heart rate variability in low-risk prematurely born infants reaching normal term: A comparison with full-term newborns. Early Hum Dev. 1993;32:183-95. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0378-3782(93)90011-I. [PubMed: 8486120]
100.
Eiselt M, Zwiener U, Witte H, Curzi-Dascalova L. Influence of prematurity and extrauterine development on the sleep state dependant heart rate patterns. Somnologie. 2002;6(3):116-23. https://doi​.org/10.1046/j​.1439-054X.2002.02189.x.
101.
Patural H, Barthelemy JC, Pichot V, Mazzocchi C, Teyssier G, Damon G, et al. Birth prematurity determines prolonged autonomic nervous system immaturity. Clin Auton Res. 2004;14:391-5. https://doi​.org/10.1007​/s10286-004-0216-9. [PubMed: 15666067]
102.
Patural H, Pichot V, Jaziri F, Teyssier G, Gaspoz JM, Roche F, et al. Autonomic cardiac control of very preterm newborns: A prolonged dysfunction. Early Hum Dev. 2008;84(10):681-7. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.earlhumdev.2008.04.010. [PubMed: 18556151]
103.
Longin E, Gerstner T, Schaible T, Lanz T, Konig S. Maturation of the autonomic nervous system: Differences in heart rate variability in premature vs term infants. J Perinat Med. 2006;34:303-8. https://doi​.org/10.1515/JPM.2006.058. [PubMed: 16856820]
104.
Witcombe NB, Yiallourou SR, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Delayed blood pressure recovery after head-up tilting during sleep in preterm infants. J Sleep Res. 2010(19):93-102. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-2869.2009.00793.x. [PubMed: 19895423]
105.
Witcombe NB, Yiallourou SR, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Blood pressure and heart rate patterns during sleep are altered in preterm-born infants: Implications for sudden infant death syndrome. Pediatrics. 2008;122(6):1242-8. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1400. [PubMed: 19047224]
106.
Witcombe NB, Yiallourou SR, Sands SA, Walker AM, Horne RS. Preterm birth alters the maturation of baroreflex sensitivity in sleeping infants. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e89-96. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1504. [PubMed: 22157139]
107.
Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Odoi A, Walker AM, Horne RS. The effect of gestational age at birth on post-term maturation of heart rate variability. Sleep. 2015;38(10):1635-44. https://doi​.org/10.5665/sleep.5064. [PMC free article: PMC4576338] [PubMed: 25902805]
108.
Yiallourou SR, Witcombe NB, Sands SA, Walker AM, Horne RS. The development of autonomic cardiovascular control is altered by preterm birth. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89(3):145-52. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.earlhumdev.2012.09.009. [PubMed: 23058299]
109.
Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Odoi A, Walker AM, Horne RS. Gestational age at birth affects maturation of baroreflex control. J Pediatr. 2015;166(3):559-65. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.jpeds.2014.11.026. [PubMed: 25556016]
110.
Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Odoi A, Walker AM, Horne RS. Cerebral oxygenation in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):435-45. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2014-0773. [PubMed: 25157010]
111.
Fyfe K, Odoi A, Yiallourou SR, Wong F, Walker AM, Horne RS. Preterm infants exhibit greater variability in cerebrovascular control than term infants. Sleep. 2015;38(9):1411-21. https://doi​.org/10.5665/sleep.4980. [PMC free article: PMC4531409] [PubMed: 25669192]
112.
Horne RSC, Cranage SM, Chau B, Adamson TM. Effects of prematurity on arousal from sleep in the newborn infant. Pediatr Res. 2000;47:468-74. https://doi​.org/10.1203​/00006450-200004000-00010. [PubMed: 10759153]
113.
Scher MS, Steppe DA, Dahl RE, Asthana S, Guthrie RD. Comparison of EEG sleep measures in healthy full-term and preterm infants at matched conceptional ages. Sleep. 1992;15(5):442-8. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/15.5.442. [PubMed: 1455128]
114.
Richardson HL, Horne RS. Arousal from sleep pathways are affected by the prone sleeping position and preterm birth: Preterm birth, prone sleeping and arousal from sleep. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89(9):705-11. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.earlhumdev.2013.05.001. [PubMed: 23725788]
115.
Tuladhar R, Harding R, Adamson TM, Horne RSC. Comparison of postnatal development of heart rate responses to trigeminal stimulation in sleeping preterm and term infants. J Sleep Res. 2005;14:29-36. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-2869.2004.00434.x. [PubMed: 15743331]
116.
Verbeek MMA, Richardson HL, Parslow PM, Walker AM, Harding R, Horne RSC. Arousal and ventilatory responses to mild hypoxia in sleeping preterm infants. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:344-53. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-2869.2008.00653.x. [PubMed: 18503514]
117.
Horne RSC, Andrew S, Mitchell K, Sly DJ, Cranage SM, Chau B, et al. Apnoea of prematurity and arousal from sleep. Early Hum Dev. 2001;61:119-33. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0378-3782(00)00129-8. [PubMed: 11223274]
118.
Carpenter RG, Irgens LM, Blair PS, Fleming PJ, Huber J, Jorch G, et al. Sudden unexplained infant death in 20 regions in Europe: Case control study. Lancet. 2004;363:185-91. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0140-6736(03)15323-8. [PubMed: 14738790]
119.
Kohyama J. Sleep as a window on the developing brain. Curr Probl Pediatr. 1998;28(3):69-92. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0045-9380(98)80054-6. [PubMed: 9571325]
120.
Carroll JL. Developmental plasticity in respiratory control. J App Physiol. 2003;94:375-89. https://doi​.org/10.1152/japplphysiol​.00809.2002. [PubMed: 12486025]
121.
Blair PS, Sidebotham P, Berry PJ, Evans M, Fleming PJ. Major epidemiological changes in sudden infant death syndrome: A 20-year population-based study in the UK. Lancet. 2006;367:314-19. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0140-6736(06)67968-3. [PubMed: 16443038]
122.
Hunt CE, Hauck FR. Sudden infant death syndrome. CMAJ. 2006;174(13):1861-9. https://doi​.org/10.1503/cmaj.051671. [PMC free article: PMC1475900] [PubMed: 16785462]
123.
Mollborg P, Alm B. Sudden infant death syndrome during low incidence in Sweden 1997-2005. Acta Paediatrica. 2010;99(1):94-8. [PubMed: 19878129]
124.
Blair PS, Sidebotham P, Evason-Coombe C, Edmonds M, Heckstall-Smith EMA, Fleming P. Hazardous co-sleeping environments and risk factors amenable to change: Case-control study of SIDS in south west England. BMJ. 2009;339:b3666. https://doi​.org/10.1136/bmj.b3666. [PMC free article: PMC2762037] [PubMed: 19826174]
125.
Gagnon R, Campbell K, Hunse C, Patrick J. Patterns of human fetal heart rate accelerations from 26 weeks to term. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1987;157:743-8. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0002-9378(87)80042-X. [PubMed: 3631176]
126.
Karin J, Hirsch M, Akselrod S. An estimate of fetal autonomic state by spectral analysis of fetal heart rate fluctuations. Pediatr Res. 1993;34:134-8. https://doi​.org/10.1203​/00006450-199308000-00005. [PubMed: 8233713]
127.
Wakai RT. Assessment of fetal neurodevelopment via fetal magnetocardiography. Exp Neurol. 2004;190:S65-S71. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.expneurol.2004.04.019. [PubMed: 15498544]
128.
Harper R, Hoppenbrouwers T, Sterman M, McGinty D, Hodgman J. Polygraphic studies of normal infants during the first six months of life. I. Heart rate and variability as a function of state. Pediatr Res. 1976;10:945-51. https://doi​.org/10.1203​/00006450-197611000-00008. [PubMed: 185576]
129.
de Beer NA, Andriessen P, Berendsen RC, Oei SG, Wijn PF, Oetomo SB. Customized spectral band analysis compared with conventional Fourier analysis of heart rate variability in neonates. Physiol Meas. 2004;25(6):1385-95. https://doi​.org/10.1088​/0967-3334/25/6/004. [PubMed: 15712717]
130.
Andriessen P, Janssen B, Berendsen RC, Oetomo SB, Wijn PF, Blanco CE. Cardiovascular autonomic regulation in preterm infants: The effect of atropine. Pediatr Res. 2004;56(6):939-46. https://doi​.org/10.1203/01​.PDR.0000145257.75072.BB. [PubMed: 15470200]
131.
Waldman S, Krauss AN, Auld PA. Baroreceptors in preterm infants: Their relationship to maturity and disease. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1979;21(6):714-22. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1469-8749.1979.tb01692.x. [PubMed: 520708]
132.
Gournay V, Drouin E, Roze JC. Development of baroreflex control of heart rate in preterm and full term Infants. Arch Dis Childhood Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2002;86(3):151-4. https://doi​.org/10.1136/fn.86.3.F151. [PMC free article: PMC1721399] [PubMed: 11978743]
133.
Task Force on Blood Pressure Control in Children. Report of the second task force on blood pressure control in children — 1987. Pediatrics. 1987;19(1):1-25.
134.
Yiallourou SR, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Prone sleeping impairs circulatory control during sleep in healthy term infants; implications for sudden infant death syndrome. Sleep. 2008;31(8):1139-46. [PMC free article: PMC2542960] [PubMed: 18714786]
135.
Wong FY, Witcombe NB, Yiallourou SR, Yorkston S, Dymowski AR, Krishnan L, et al. Cerebral oxygenation is depressed during sleep in healthy term infants when they sleep prone. Pediatrics. 2011;127(3):e558-65. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2010-2724. [PubMed: 21357341]
136.
Andriessen P, Oetomo SB, Peters C, Vermeulen B, Wijn PFF, Blanco CE. Baroreceptor reflex sensitivity in human neonates: The effect of postmenstrual age. J Physiol. 2005;568:333-41. https://doi​.org/10.1113/jphysiol​.2005.093641. [PMC free article: PMC1474770] [PubMed: 16051623]
137.
Yiallourou SR, Sands SA, Walker AM, Horne RS. Maturation of heart rate and blood pressure variability during sleep in term-born infants. Sleep. 2012;35(2):177-86. https://doi​.org/10.5665/sleep.1616. [PMC free article: PMC3250356] [PubMed: 22294807]
138.
Yiallourou SR, Sands SA, Walker AM, Horne RS. Postnatal development of baroreflex sensitivity in infancy. J Physiol. 2010;588(Pt 12):2193-203. https://doi​.org/10.1113/jphysiol​.2010.187070. [PMC free article: PMC2911220] [PubMed: 20421281]
139.
Cohen G, Malcolm G, Henderson-Smart D. Ventilatory response of the newborn infant to mild hypoxia. Pediatr Pulmonol. 1997;24:163-72. https://doi​.org/10.1002​/(SICI)1099-0496(199709)24​:3<163​::AID-PPUL1>3.0.CO;2-O. [PubMed: 9330412]
140.
Martin RJ, DiFiore JM, Jana L, Davis RL, Miller MJ, Coles SK, et al. Persistence of the biphasic ventilatory response hypoxia in preterm infants. J Pediatr. 1998;132(6):960-4. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(98)70391-9. [PubMed: 9627586]
141.
Parslow PM, Cranage SM, Adamson TM, Harding R, Horne RSC. Arousal and ventilatory responses to hypoxia in sleeping infants: Effects of maternal smoking. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2004;140:77-87. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.resp.2004.01.004. [PubMed: 15109930]
142.
Parslow PM, Harding R, Cranage SM, Adamson TM, Horne RSC. Ventilatory responses preceding hypoxia-induced arousal in infants: Effects of sleep-state. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2003;136:235-47. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S1569-9048(03)00085-5. [PubMed: 12853014]
143.
Trinder J, Newman NM, Le Grande M, Whitworth F, Kay A, Pirkis J, et al. Behavioral and EEG responses to auditory stimuli during sleep in newborn infants and in infants aged 3 months. Biological Psychology. 1990;90:213-27. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0301-0511(90)90035-U. [PubMed: 2132679]
144.
Parslow PM, Harding R, Cranage SM, Adamson TM, Horne RSC. Arousal responses to somatosensory and mild hypoxic stimuli are depressed during quiet sleep in healthy term infants. Sleep. 2003;26(6):739-44. [PubMed: 14572129]
145.
Montemitro E, Franco P, Scaillet S, Kato I, Groswasser J, Villa MP, et al. Maturation of spontaneous arousals in healthy infants. Sleep. 2008;31(1):47-54. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/31.1.47. [PMC free article: PMC2225547] [PubMed: 18220077]
146.
Richardson HL, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Sleep position alters arousal processes maximally at the high-risk age for sudden infant death syndrome. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:450-7. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-2869.2008.00683.x. [PubMed: 19090953]
147.
Blackwell C, Moscovis S, Hall S, Burns C, Scott RJ. Exploring the risk factors for sudden infant deaths and their role in inflammatory responses to infection. Front Immunol. 2015;6:44. https://doi​.org/10.3389/fimmu.2015.00044. [PMC free article: PMC4350416] [PubMed: 25798137]
148.
Galland BC, Elder DE. Sudden unexpected death in infancy: Biological mechanisms. Paediatr Respir Rev. 2014;15(4):287-92. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.prrv.2014.09.003. [PubMed: 25301029]
149.
Oyen H, Markstead T, Skjaerven R, Irgens LM, Helweg-Larsen K, Alm B, et al. Combined effects of sleeping position and the perinatal risk factors in sudden infant death syndrome: The Nordic epidemiological study. Pediatrics. 1997;100(4):613-21. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.100.4.613. [PubMed: 9310514]
150.
Mitchell EA. Sleeping position of infants and the sudden infant death syndrome. Acta Paediatrica. 1993;389:26-30. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.1993.tb12870.x. [PubMed: 8374186]
151.
Ponsonby AL, Dwyer T. The Tasmanian SIDS case-control study: Univariate and multivariate risk factor analysis. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 1995;9:256-72. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-3016.1995.tb00141.x. [PubMed: 7479275]
152.
Taylor JA, Krieger JW, Reay DT, David RL, Harruff R, Cheney LK. Prone sleeping position and sudden infant death syndrome in King County, Washington: A case control study. Pediatrics. 1996;128:626-30. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(96)80126-0. [PubMed: 8627433]
153.
Beal SM, Finch CF. An overview of retrospective case-control studies investigating the relationship between prone sleeping position and SIDS. J Paediatr Child Health. 1991;27:334-9. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1440-1754.1991.tb00414.x. [PubMed: 1836736]
154.
Fleming PJ, Gilbert R, Azaz Y, Berry PJ, Rudd PT, Stewart A, et al. Interaction between bedding and sleeping position in the sudden infant death syndrome: A population based case-control study. BMJ. 1990;301(6743):85-9. https://doi​.org/10.1136/bmj.301.6743.85. [PMC free article: PMC1663432] [PubMed: 2390588]
155.
Galland BC, Reeves H, Taylor B, Bolton DPG. Sleep position, autonomic function, and arousal. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1998;78:189-94. https://doi​.org/10.1136/fn.78.3.F189. [PMC free article: PMC1720791] [PubMed: 9713030]
156.
Horne R, Ferens D, Watts A, Vitkovic J, Lacey B, Andrew S, et al. The prone sleeping position impairs arousability in term infants. J Pediatr. 2001;138:811-16. https://doi​.org/10.1067/mpd.2001.114475. [PubMed: 11391321]
157.
Galland B, Taylor B, Bolton D, Sayers R. Vasoconstriction following spontaneous sighs and head-up tilts in infants sleeping prone and supine. Early Hum Dev. 2000;58:119-32. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0378-3782(00)00070-0. [PubMed: 10854799]
158.
Ariagno RL, Mirmiran M, Adams MM, Saporito AG, Dubin AM, Baldwin RB. Effect of position on sleep, heart rate variability, and QT interval in preterm infants at 1 and 3 months’ corrected age. Pediatrics. 2003;111:622-5. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.111.3.622. [PubMed: 12612246]
159.
Sahni R, Schulz H, Kashyap S, Ohira-Kist K, Fifer WP, Myers MM. Postural differences in cardiac dynamics during quiet and active sleep in low birthweight infants. Acta Paediatrica. 1999;88:1396-401. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.1999.tb01058.x. [PubMed: 10626529]
160.
Gabai N, Cohen A, Mahagney A, Bader D, Tirosh E. Arterial blood flow and autonomic function in full-term infants. Clin Physiol Funct Imaging. 2006;26:127-31. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1475-097X.2006.00661.x. [PubMed: 16494604]
161.
Kahn A, Grosswasser J, Sottiaux M, Rebuffat E, Franco P, Dramaix M. Prone or supine body position and sleep characteristics in infants. Pediatrics. 1993;91:1112-15. [PubMed: 8502511]
162.
Chong A, Murphy N, Matthews T. Effect of prone sleeping on circulatory control in infants. Arch Dis Child. 2000;82:253-6. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.82.3.253. [PMC free article: PMC1718241] [PubMed: 10685934]
163.
Ammari A, Schulze KF, Ohira-Kist K, Kashyap S, Fifer WP, Myers MM, et al. Effects of body position on thermal, cardiorespiratory and metabolic activity in low birth weight infants. Early Hum Dev. 2009;85:497-501. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.earlhumdev.2009.04.005. [PMC free article: PMC2719968] [PubMed: 19419824]
164.
Franco P, Grosswasser J, Sottiaux M, Broadfield E, Kahn A. Decreased cardiac responses to auditory stimulation during prone sleep. Pediatrics. 1996;97:174-8. [PubMed: 8584373]
165.
Tuladhar R, Harding R, Cranage SM, Adamson TM, Horne RSC. Effects of sleep position, sleep state and age on heart rate responses following provoked arousal in term infants. Early Hum Dev. 2003;71:157-69. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0378-3782(03)00005-7. [PubMed: 12663153]
166.
Wong F, Yiallourou SR, Odoi A, Browne P, Walker AM, Horne RS. Cerebrovascular control is altered in healthy term infants when they sleep prone. Sleep. 2013;36(12):1911-18. https://doi​.org/10.5665/sleep.3228. [PMC free article: PMC3825441] [PubMed: 24293766]
167.
Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Horne RS. The development of cardiovascular and cerebral vascular control in preterm infants. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;18(4):299-310. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.smrv.2013.06.002. [PubMed: 23907095]
168.
Ariagno R, van Liempt S, Mirmiran M. Fewer spontaneous arousals during prone sleep in preterm infants at 1 and 3 months corrected age. J Perinatol. 2006;26:306-12. https://doi​.org/10.1038/sj.jp.7211490. [PubMed: 16572196]
169.
Goto K, Maeda T, Mirmiran M, Ariagno R. Effects of prone and supine position on sleep characteristics in preterm infants. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;53:315-17. https://doi​.org/10.1046/j​.1440-1819.1999.00549.x. [PubMed: 10459722]
170.
Bhat RY, Hannam S, Pressler R, Rafferty GF, Peacock JL, Greenough A. Effect of prone and supine position on sleep, apneas, and arousal in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2006;118:101-7. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1873. [PubMed: 16818554]
171.
Kato I, Scaillet S, Groswasser J, Montemitro E, Togari H, Lin J, et al. Spontaneous arousability in prone and supine position in healthy infants. Sleep. 2006;29(6):785-90. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/29.6.785. [PubMed: 16796217]
172.
Franco P, Pardou A, Hassid S, Lurquin P, Groswasser J, Kahn A. Auditory arousal thresholds are higher when infants sleep in the prone position. J Pediatr. 1998;132:240-3. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0022-3476(98)70438-X. [PubMed: 9506634]
173.
Horne RSC, Bandopadhayay P, Vitkovic J, Cranage SM, Adamson TM. Effects of age and sleeping position on arousal from sleep in preterm infants. Sleep. 2002;25:746-50. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/25.7.746. [PubMed: 12405610]
174.
Richardson HL, Walker AM, Horne R. Stimulus type does not affect infant arousal response patterns. J Sleep Res. 2010;19:111-15. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1365-2869.2009.00764.x. [PubMed: 19691474]
175.
Ponsonby A, Dwyer T, Gibbons LE, Cochrane JA, Jones ME, McCall MJ. Thermal environment and sudden infant death syndrome: Case-control study. BMJ. 1992;304:279-91. https://doi​.org/10.1136/bmj.304.6822.277. [PMC free article: PMC1881052] [PubMed: 1739826]
176.
Tuffnell CS, Peterson SA, Wailoo MP. Prone sleeping infants have a reduced ability to lose heat. Early Human Dev. 1995;43:109-16. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0378-3782(95)01659-7. [PubMed: 8903756]
177.
Skadberg BT, Markstead T. Behavior and physiological responses during prone and supine sleep in early infancy. Arch Dis Child. 1997;76:320-4. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.76.4.320. [PMC free article: PMC1717149] [PubMed: 9166023]
178.
Bolton DPG, Nelson EAS, Taylor BJ, Weatherall IL. Thermal balance in infants. J Appl Physiol. 1996;80(6):2234-42. [PubMed: 8806935]
179.
L’Hoir MP, Engelberts AC, van Well GTJ, McClelland S, Westers P, Dandachli T, et al. Risk and preventive factors for cot death in The Netherlands, a low-incidence country. Eur J Pediatr. 1998;157:681-8. https://doi​.org/10.1007/s004310050911. [PubMed: 9727856]
180.
Kahn A, Wachholder A, Winkler M, Rebuffat E. Prospective study on the prevalence of sudden infant death and possible risk factors in Brussels: Preliminary results (1987-1988). Eur J Pediatr. 1990;149:284-6. https://doi​.org/10.1007/BF02106296. [PubMed: 2303079]
181.
Williams SM, Taylor BJ, Mitchell EA, & Other members of the National Cot Death Study Group. Sudden infant death syndrome: Insulation from bedding and clothing and its effect modifiers. Int J Epidemiology. 1996;25(2):366-75. https://doi​.org/10.1093/ije/25.2.366. [PubMed: 9119562]
182.
Naeye RL, Ladis B, Drage JS. Sudden infant death syndrome: A prospective study. Am J Dis Child. 1976;130:1207-10. https://doi​.org/10.1001/archpedi​.1976.02120120041005. [PubMed: 984002]
183.
Bach V, Bouferrache B, Kremp O, Maingourd Y, Libert JP. Regulation of sleep and body temperature in response to exposure to cool and warm environments in neonates. Pediatrics. 1994;93(5):789-96. [PubMed: 8165080]
184.
Franco P, Scaillet S, Valente F, Chabanski S, Groswasser J, Kahn A. Ambient temperature is associated with changes in infants’ arousability from sleep. Sleep. 2001;24:325-9. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/24.3.325. [PubMed: 11322716]
185.
Stephan-Blanchard E, Chardon K, Leke A, Delanaud S, Bach V, Telliez F. Heart rate variability in sleeping preterm neonates exposed to cool and warm thermal conditions. PloS One. 2013;8(7):e68211. https://doi​.org/10.1371/journal​.pone.0068211. [PMC free article: PMC3698119] [PubMed: 23840888]
186.
Blair PS, Mitchell EA, Heckstall-Smith EMA, Fleming PJ. Head covering a major modifiable risk factor for sudden infant death syndrome: A systematic review. Arch Dis Child. 2008;93:778-83. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.2007.136366. [PubMed: 18450800]
187.
Mitchell EA, Thompson JM, Becroft DM, Bajanowski T, Brinkmann B, Happe A, et al. Head covering and the risk for SIDS: Findings from the New Zealand and German SIDS case-control studies. Pediatrics. 2008;121(6):e1478-e83. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2749. [PubMed: 18519451]
188.
Paluszynska DA, Harris KA, Thach BT. Influence of sleep position experience on ability of prone-sleeping infants to escape from asphyxiating microenvironments by changing head position. Pediatrics. 2004;114(6):1634-9. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2004-0754. [PubMed: 15574627]
189.
Franco P, Lipshut W, Valente F, Adams M, Grosswasser J, Kahn A. Cardiac autonomic characteristics in infants sleeping with their head covered by bedclothes. J Sleep Res. 2003;12(2):125-32. https://doi​.org/10.1046/j​.1365-2869.2003.00340.x. [PubMed: 12753349]
190.
Franco P, Lipshutz W, Valente F, Adams S, Scaillet S, Kahn A. Decreased arousals in infants who sleep with the face covered by bedclothes. Pediatrics. 2002;109(6):1112-17. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.109.6.1112. [PubMed: 12042551]
191.
Carpenter R, McGarvey C, Mitchell EA, Tappin DM, Vennemann MM, Smuk M, et al. Bed sharing when parents do not smoke: Is there a risk of SIDS? An individual level analysis of five major case-control studies. BMJ Open. 2013;3(5):e002299. https://doi​.org/10.1136​/bmjopen-2012-002299. [PMC free article: PMC3657670] [PubMed: 23793691]
192.
Blair PS, Sidebotham P, Pease A, Fleming PJ. Bed-sharing in the absence of hazardous circumstances: Is there a risk of sudden infant death syndrome? An analysis from two case-control studies conducted in the UK. PloS One. 2014;9(9):e107799. https://doi​.org/10.1371/journal​.pone.0107799. [PMC free article: PMC4169572] [PubMed: 25238618]
193.
Escott AS, Elder DE, Zuccollo JM. Sudden unexpected infant death and bedsharing: Referrals to the Wellington Coroner 1997-2006. N Z Med J. 2009;122(1298):59-68. [PubMed: 19680305]
194.
Hauck FR, Signore C, Fein SB, Raju TNK. Infant sleeping arrangements and practices during the first year of life. Pediatrics. 2008;122:S113-20. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1315o. [PubMed: 18829826]
195.
McKenna JJ, Mosko SS. Sleep and arousal synchrony and independence among mothers and infants sleeping apart and together (same bed): An experiment in evolutionary medicine. Acta Paediatr Suppl. 1994;397:94-102. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.1994.tb13271.x. [PubMed: 7981481]
196.
Baddock SA, Galland BC, Bolton DP, Williams SM, Taylor BJ. Differences in infant and parent behaviors during routine bed sharing compared with cot sleeping in the home setting. Pediatrics. 2006;117(5):1599-607. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1636. [PubMed: 16651313]
197.
Lipton EL, Steinschneider A, Richmond JB. Swaddling, a child care practice: Historical, cultural, and experimental observations. Pediatrics. 1965;35:521-67. [PubMed: 14248049]
198.
van Sleuwen BE, L’Hoir MP, Engleberts AC, Westers P, Schulpen TWJ. Infant care practices related to cot death in Turkish and Moroccan families in the Netherlands. Arch Dis Child. 2003;88:784-8. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.88.9.784. [PMC free article: PMC1719636] [PubMed: 12937097]
199.
Beal SM, Porter C. Sudden infant death syndrome related to climate. Acta Paediatrica Scand. 1991;80:278-87. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.1991.tb11850.x. [PubMed: 2035322]
200.
Ponsonby A-L, Dwyer T, Gibbons LE, Cochrane JA, Wang Y-G. Factors potentiating the risk of sudden infant death syndrome associated with the prone position. New Eng J Med. 1993;329(6):377-82. https://doi​.org/10.1056​/NEJM199308053290601. [PubMed: 8326970]
201.
Wilson CA, Taylor BJ, Laing RM, Williams SM, Mitchell EA. Clothing and bedding and its relevance to sudden infant death syndrome: Further results from the New Zealand Cot Death Study. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health. 1994;30:506-12. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1440-1754.1994.tb00722.x. [PubMed: 7865263]
202.
Karp H. The happiest baby on the block. New York: Bantam, 2002.
203.
van Sleuwen BE, Engelberts AC, Boere-Boonekamp MM, Kuis W, Schulpen TWJ, L’Hoir MP. Swaddling: A systematic review. Pediatrics. 2007;120:e1097-106. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2006-2083. [PubMed: 17908730]
204.
Li Y, Liu J, Liu F, Guo G, Anme T, Ushijima H. Maternal child-rearing behaviors and correlates in rural minority areas of Yunnan, China. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2000;21:114-22. https://doi​.org/10.1097​/00004703-200004000-00005. [PubMed: 10791479]
205.
Caglayan S, Yaprak I, Seckin E, Kansoy S, Aydinlioglu H. A different approach to sleep problems of infancy: Swaddling above the waist. Turk J Pediatr. 1991;33(2):117-20. [PubMed: 1844180]
206.
Urnaa V, Kizuki M, Nakamura K, Kaneko A, Inose T, Seino K, et al. Association of swaddling, rickets onset and bone properties in children in Ulaanbaatar, Mongolia. Public Health. 2006;120:834-40. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.puhe.2006.05.009. [PubMed: 16872650]
207.
Bystrova K, Matthiesen AS, Widstrom AM, Ransjo-Arvidson AB, Welles-Nystrom B, Vorontsov I, et al. The effect of Russian Maternity Home routines on breastfeeding and neonatal weight loss with special reference to swaddling. Early Hum Dev. 2007;83:29-39. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.earlhumdev.2006.03.016. [PubMed: 16716541]
208.
Pease AS, Fleming PJ, Hauck FR, Moon RY, Horne RS, L’Hoir MP, et al. Swaddling and the risk of sudden infant death syndrome: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2016;137(6):e20153275. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2015-3275. [PubMed: 27244847]
209.
Moon RY, & Task Force On Sudden Infant Death Syndrome. SIDS and other sleep-related infant deaths: Evidence base for 2016 updated recommendations for a safe infant sleeping environment. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20162940. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2940. [PubMed: 27940805]
210.
Gerard CM, Harris KA, Thach BT. Physiologic studies on swaddling: An ancient child care practice, which may promote the supine position for infant sleep. J Pediatr. 2002;141(3):398-403. https://doi​.org/10.1067/mpd.2002.127508. [PubMed: 12219062]
211.
Richardson HL, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Minimizing the risk of sudden infant death syndrome: To swaddle or not to swaddle? J Pediatr. 2009;155:475-81. [PubMed: 19540517]
212.
Narangerel G, Pollock J, Manaseki-Holland S, Henderson J. The effects of swaddling on oxygen saturation and respiratory rate of healthy infants in Mongolia. Acta Paediatrica. 2007;96:261-5. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.2007.00123.x. [PubMed: 17429917]
213.
Franco P, Seret N, van Hees J, Scaillet S, Grosswasser J, Kahn A. Influence of swaddling on sleep and arousal characteristics of healthy infants. Pediatrics. 2005;115:1307-11. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2004-1460. [PubMed: 15867039]
214.
Richardson HL, Walker AM, Horne RS. Influence of swaddling experience on spontaneous arousal patterns and autonomic control in sleeping infants. J Pediatr. 2010;157(1):85-91. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.jpeds.2010.01.005. [PubMed: 20227720]
215.
Chisholm JS. Swaddling, cradleboards and the development of children. Early Hum Dev. 1978;2(3):255-75. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0378-3782(78)90029-4. [PubMed: 551929]
216.
Gerard CM, Harris KA, Thach BT. Spontaneous arousals in supine infants while swaddled and unswaddled during rapid eye movement and quiet sleep. Pediatrics. 2002;110(6):70-6. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.110.6.e70. [PubMed: 12456937]
217.
Franco P, Scaillet S, Grosswasser J, Kahn A. Increased cardiac autonomic responses to auditory challenges in swaddled infants. Sleep. 2004;27(8):1527-32. https://doi​.org/10.1093/sleep/27.8.1527. [PubMed: 15683144]
218.
Hoffman HJ, Damus K, Hillman L, Krongrad E. Risk factors for SIDS. Results of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development SIDS cooperative epidemiological study. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1988;533:13-30. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1749-6632.1988.tb37230.x. [PubMed: 3048169]
219.
Leach CE, Blair PS, Fleming PJ, Smith IJ, Platt MW, Berry PJ, et al. Epidemiology of SIDS and explained sudden infant deaths. Pediatrics. 1999;104:43-53. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.104.4.e43. [PubMed: 10506268]
220.
Heininger U, Kleemann WJ, Cherry JD, & Sudden Infant Death Syndrome Study Group. A controlled study of the relationship between bordetella pertussis infections and sudden unexplained deaths among German infants. Pediatrics. 2004;114(1):9-15. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.114.1.e9. [PubMed: 15231967]
221.
Horne RS, Osborne A, Vitkovic J, Lacey B, Andrew S, Chau B, et al. Arousal from sleep in infants is impaired following an infection. Early Hum Dev. 2002;66(2):89-100. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/S0378-3782(01)00237-7. [PubMed: 11872313]
222.
Vennemann MM, Bajanowski T, Brinkmann B, Jorch G, Yucsesan K, Sauerland C, et al. Does breastfeeding reduce the risk of sudden infant death syndrome? Pediatrics. 2009;123(3):e406-10. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2145. [PubMed: 19254976]
223.
Hauck FR, Thompson JM, Tanabe KO, Moon RY, Vennemann MM. Breastfeeding and reduced risk of sudden infant death syndrome: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2011;128(1):103-10. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2010-3000. [PubMed: 21669892]
224.
Gordon AE, Saadi AT, MacKenzie DAC, Molony N, James VS, Weir DM, et al. The protective effect of breast feeding in relation to sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS): III. Detection of IgA antibodies in human milk that bind to bacterial toxins implicated in SIDS. FEMS Immunol Med Microbiol. 1999;25(1-2):175-82. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1574-695X.1999.tb01341.x. [PubMed: 10443506]
225.
McVea KLSP, Turner PD, Peppler DK. The role of breastfeeding in sudden infant death syndrome. J Human Lactation. 2000;16(1):13-20. https://doi​.org/10.1177​/089033440001600104. [PubMed: 11138219]
226.
Butte NF, Smith EO, Garza C. Heart rate of breast-fed and formula-fed infants. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 1991;13(4):391-6. https://doi​.org/10.1097​/00005176-199111000-00009. [PubMed: 1779313]
227.
Elias MF, Nicolson NA, Bora C, Johnston J. Sleep/wake patterns of breast-fed infants in the first 2 years of life. Pediatrics. 1986;77(3):322-9. [PubMed: 3951913]
228.
Horne R, Franco P, Adamson T, Grosswasser J, Kahn A. Influences of maternal cigarette smoking on infant arousability. Early Hum Dev. 2004;79(1):49-58. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.earlhumdev.2004.04.005. [PubMed: 15282122]
229.
Fleming PJ, Blair PS, Pollard K, Platt MW, Leach C, Smith I, et al. Pacifier use and sudden infant death syndrome: Results from the CESDI/SUDI case control study. Arch Dis Child. 1999;81:112-16. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.81.2.112. [PMC free article: PMC1718026] [PubMed: 10490514]
230.
Hauck FR, Omojokun OO, Siadaty MS. Do pacifiers reduce the risk of sudden infant death syndrome? A meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2005;116:e716-23. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2004-2631. [PubMed: 16216900]
231.
Li D, Willinger M, Petiti DB, Odouli R, Liu L, Hoffman HJ. Use of a dummy (pacifier) during sleep and risk of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS): Population based case-control study. BMJ. 2006;332:18-22. https://doi​.org/10.1136/bmj​.38671.640475.55. [PMC free article: PMC1325127] [PubMed: 16339767]
232.
Mitchell EA, Blair PS, L’Hoir MP. Should pacifiers be recommended to prevent sudden infant death syndrome? Pediatrics. 2006;117(5):1755-8. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1625. [PubMed: 16651334]
233.
Vennemann MM, Bajanowski T, Brinkmann B, Jorch G, Sauerland C, Mitchell EA, et al. Sleep environment risk factors for sudden infant death syndrome: The German Sudden Infant Death Syndrome Study. Pediatrics. 2009;123(4):1162-70. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2008-0505. [PubMed: 19336376]
234.
Horne RS, Hauck FR, Moon RY, L’Hoir MP, Blair PS, Physiology and Epidemiology Working Groups of the International Society for the Study and Prevention of Perinatal and Infant Death. Dummy (pacifier) use and sudden infant death syndrome: Potential advantages and disadvantages. J Paediatr Child Health. 2014;50(3):170-4. https://doi​.org/10.1111/jpc.12402. [PubMed: 24674245]
235.
Franco P, Chabanski S, Scaillet S, Grosswasser J, Kahn A. Pacifier use modifies infant’s cardiac autonomic controls during sleep. Early Hum Dev. 2004;77:99-108. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.earlhumdev.2004.02.002. [PubMed: 15113636]
236.
Yiallourou SR, Poole H, Prathivadi P, Odoi A, Wong FY, Horne RS. The effects of dummy/pacifier use on infant blood pressure and autonomic activity during sleep. Sleep Med. 2014;15(12):1508-16. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.sleep.2014.07.011. [PubMed: 25441754]
237.
Horne RS, Fyfe KL, Odoi A, Athukoralage A, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY. Dummy/pacifier use in preterm infants increases blood pressure and improves heart rate control. Pediatr Res. 2016;79(2):325-32. https://doi​.org/10.1038/pr.2015.212. [PubMed: 26488553]
238.
Lappi H, Valkonen-Korhonen M, Georgiadis S, Tarvainen MP, Tarkka IM, Karjalainen PA, et al. Effects of nutritive and non-nutritive sucking on infant heart rate variability during the first 6 months of life. Infant Behav Dev. 2007;30:546-56. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.infbeh.2007.04.005. [PubMed: 17568681]
239.
Hanzer M, Zotter H, Sauseng W, Pichler G, Mueller W, Kerbl R. Non-nutritive sucking habits in sleeping infants. Neonatol. 2010;97:61-6. https://doi​.org/10.1159/000231518. [PubMed: 19648773]
240.
Cohen M, Brown DR, Myers MM. Cardiovascular responses to pacifier experience and feeding in newborn infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2001;39:34-9. https://doi​.org/10.1002/dev.1025. [PubMed: 11507707]
241.
Franco P, Scaillet S, Wermenbol V, Valente F, Groswasser J, Kahn A. The influence of a pacifier on infants’ arousals from sleep. J Pediatr. 2000;136:775-9. [PubMed: 10839876]
242.
Hanzer M, Zotter H, Sauseng W, Pichler G, Pfurtscheller K, Mueller W, et al. Pacifier use does not alter the frequency or duration of spontaneous arousals in sleeping infants. Sleep Med. 2009;10:464-70. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.sleep.2008.03.014. [PubMed: 18684666]
243.
Odoi A, Andrew S, Wong FY, Yiallourou SR, Horne RS. Pacifier use does not alter sleep and spontaneous arousal patterns in healthy term-born infants. Acta Paediatrica. 2014;103(12):1244-50. https://doi​.org/10.1111/apa.12790. [PubMed: 25169652]
244.
Tonkin SL, Lui D, McIntosh CG, Rowley S, Knight DB, Gunn AJ. Effect of pacifier use on mandibular position in preterm infants. Acta Paediatrica. 2007;96(10):1433-6. https://doi​.org/10.1111/j​.1651-2227.2007.00444.x. [PubMed: 17714544]
245.
Cozzi F, Albani R, Cardi E. A common pathophysiology for sudden cot death and sleep apnoea. “The vacuum-glossoptosis syndrome”. Med Hypoth. 1979;5(3):329-38. https://doi​.org/10.1016​/0306-9877(79)90013-6. [PubMed: 223019]
246.
Stratton K, Almario DA, Wizemann TM, McCormick MC. Immunization safety review: Vaccinations and sudden unexpected death in infancy. Washington DC: National Academies Press, 2003. [PubMed: 25057654]
247.
Mitchell EA, Stewart AW, Clements M. Immunisation and the sudden infant death syndrome. New Zealand Cot Death Study Group. Arch Dis Child. 1995;73(6):498-501. https://doi​.org/10.1136/adc.73.6.498. [PMC free article: PMC1511439] [PubMed: 8546503]
248.
Jonville-Bera AP, Autret-Leca E, Barbeillon F, Paris-Llado J. Sudden unexpected death in infants under 3 months of age and vaccination status — A case-control study. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2001;51(3):271-6. https://doi​.org/10.1046/j​.1365-2125.2001.00341.x. [PMC free article: PMC2015026] [PubMed: 11298074]
249.
Fleming PJ, Blair PS, Platt MW, Tripp J, Smith IJ, Golding J. The UK accelerated immunisation programme and sudden unexpected death in infancy: Case-control study. BMJ. 2001;322(7290):822. https://doi​.org/10.1136/bmj.322.7290.822. [PMC free article: PMC30557] [PubMed: 11290634]
250.
Vennemann MM, Butterfass-Bahloul T, Jorch G, Brinkmann B, Findeisen M, Sauerland C, et al. Sudden infant death syndrome: No increased risk after immunisation. Vaccine. 2007;25(2):336-40. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.vaccine.2006.07.027. [PubMed: 16945457]
251.
Vennemann MM, Hoffgen M, Bajanowski T, Hense HW, Mitchell EA. Do immunisations reduce the risk for SIDS? A meta-analysis. Vaccine. 2007;25(26):4875-9. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.vaccine.2007.02.077. [PubMed: 17400342]
252.
Loy CS, Horne RS, Read PA, Cranage SM, Chau B, Adamson TM. Immunization has no effect on arousal from sleep in the newborn infant. J Paediatr Child Health. 1998;34(4):349-54. https://doi​.org/10.1046/j​.1440-1754.1998.00244.x. [PubMed: 9727177]
253.
Leiter JC, Bohm I. Mechanisms of pathogenesis in the sudden infant death syndrome. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2007;159:127-38. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.resp.2007.05.014. [PubMed: 17644048]
254.
Chang RR, Keens TG, Rodriguez S, Chen AY. Sudden infant death syndrome: Changing epidemiologic patterns in California 1989-2004. J Pediatr. 2008;153(4):498-502. https://doi​.org/10.1016/j​.jpeds.2008.04.022. [PubMed: 18534214]
255.
Hauck FR, Tanabe KO. International trends in sudden infant death syndrome: Stabilization of rates requires further action. Pediatrics. 2008;122(3):660-6. https://doi​.org/10.1542/peds.2007-0135. [PubMed: 18762537]
256.
Fleming PJ, Blair PS, Pease A. Sudden unexpected death in infancy: Aetiology, pathophysiology, epidemiology and prevention in 2015. Arch Dis Child. 2015;100:984-8. https://doi​.org/10.1136​/archdischild-2014-306424. [PubMed: 25699563]
© 2018 The Contributors, with the exception of which is by Federal United States employees and is therefore in the public domain.

This work is licenced under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0) License. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0 or send a letter to Creative Commons, 444 Castro Street, Suite 900, Mountain View, California, 94041, USA. This licence allows for the copying, distribution, display and performance of this work for non-commercial purposes providing the work is clearly attributed to the copyright holders. Address all inquiries to the Director at the above address.

Bookshelf ID: NBK513398PMID: 30035963

Views

  • PubReader
  • Print View
  • Cite this Page
  • PDF version of this title (46M)

Related information

  • PMC
    PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed
    Links to PubMed

Similar articles in PubMed

See reviews...See all...

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...